Twib.in
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 1
The Evolutionary Foundations of Hierarchy:
Status, Dominance, Prestige, and Leadership
Mark van Vugt & Joshua M. Tybur
Chapter in D. M. Buss, Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (Second Edition)
August 2014 (in press)
We would like to thank Willem Frankenhuis, Michael Price, Richard Ronay and an anonymous
reviewer for their comments on an earlier version of our chapter.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 2
The Evolutionary Foundations of Hierarchy:
Status, Dominance, Prestige, and Leadership
"Who can say for sure that the deprivation which afflicts him with hunger is more painful than the
deprivation which afflicts him with envy of his neighbor's new car" – JK Galbraith
The imperial city of Rome was the largest urban area in the world of its time. Despite being a
democracy, Rome had an elaborate system of ranks and social standings visible to all. The Roman
dress, the toga, was a status symbol par excellence; only free Roman citizens could wear it.
Foreigners, slaves and even exiled citizens could not wear it in public, and it was taboo for Roman
citizens to be seen in public without wearing one. Social status within the citizen class was further
demarked by different kinds of togas. On formal occasions most Roman men and ordinary members
of the Senate wore plain white toga virilis, whereas politicians campaigning for public office wore the
conspicuously ultra-white, bleached toga candida. The white toga praetexta had a broad purple
stripe on its border, and only priests and magistrates could wear it. Finally, the toga picta, a brightly
coloured and richly embroidered garment, could only be worn by military commanders on their
triumphs through the streets of Rome and by the consuls and emperors on special occasions like the
Gladiator Games (Baker, 2010).
Rome was not unique in emphasis of status. All human societies, large or small, wealthy or
poor, industrialized or subsistence based, have status hierarchies. The anthropologist Donald Brown
(1991) has documented social status as universal across human cultures, and hierarchy is
conceptualized as one of the key, universal dimensions of human social relationships in Fiske's (1992)
Relational Model Theory. Even foraging societies that might appear egalitarian at first blush are
characterized by status hierarchies and, like the Romans, individuals at higher places in status
hierarchies enjoy special benefits. For example, adult males of the Ache, an indigenous foraging
people in Paraguay, invest large amounts of time and energy into acquiring meat. In a sense, hunting
is their job, and meat is their income. The tangible fruits of one individual's labors, though, cannot be
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 3
stored long-term in a manner that could allow the accumulation of capital and the subsequent status
increases that real estate moguls, wall street executives, and heads of state enjoy. Instead, meat is
shared with other members of the group in a relatively egalitarian manner. This egalitarianism in
terms of tangible resources does not prevent the accumulation of status based on hunting
performance, though; the best hunters in the band accumulate prestige, which allows them to have
more extramarital affairs and sire more children than the average hunter (Hill & Kaplan, 1988).
Similar associations between hunting skills and reproductive success have been reported among
other hunter-gather societies such as the Ache, Hadza, !Kung, and Tsimane (Von Rueden, 2014).
In addition to being common across human cultures, status hierarchies are prevalent in non-
humans social species as well (Ellis, 1995). A widely cited example is the pecking order in chickens. If
a group of chickens is placed together for the first time, they will all initially peck each other in
competition over food. Before long, though, a simple linear hierarchy emerges within the group
where every hen knows its place: A pecks B, B pecks C, and A, B, and C all peck D, and so on, and the
pecking order determines which hens gets preferential access to food. Hierarchy also determines
access to females in various primate species (to which humans are closely linked). There are positive
associations between male rank and reproductive success among chimpanzees, bonobos, gorillas and
rhesus macaques, although the strength of the association varies depending upon socio-ecological
conditions such as (a) resource predictability, (b) the ability to monopolize resources and (c) form
levelling coalitions against dominants (Boehm, 1999; Ellis, 1995).
Given the ubiquitous nature of status hierarchies across human and non-human groups, and
the fitness-relevant consequences of placement in status hierarchies (e.g., access to food and mates),
it is likely that natural selection would have favored psychological mechanisms that are specialized
for navigating status hierarchies. This chapter explores these mechanisms in several ways. First, we
define the relevant concepts. Then we discuss the selection pressures that might have favored the
evolution of a universal status striving tendency. We do this partially through the logic of a simple
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 4
game theoretical model. Third, we review some of the proximate mechanisms -- including
behavioral, morphological, hormonal, and affective systems -- through which individuals are able to
assess their relative status and likelihood of winning a status challenge, make status gains, and
manage status losses, whereby we pay attention to sex differences in status striving. Finally, we
investigate the emergence of one specific high-status position in human groups, leadership.
Definitions
Following Cummins (2005), we
define
status as an individual's standing in the social hierarchy
which determines priority access to resources in competitive situations. We further draw a
distinction between status hierarchies based on
dominance versus
prestige (Henrich & Gil-White,
2001; note that social psychologists Magee and Galinsky [2008] use the terms
power versus
status,
respectively, to refer to these concepts). In dominance hierarchies – which are common among
nonhuman primates – individuals achieve priority access to resources through threat, intimidation
and displays of force. Prestige, in contrast, is freely deferred status granted to individuals because
they help other individuals achieve their goals. In return, prestigious individuals (e.g., individuals with
valued skills or knowledge) receive priority access to resources (Hill & Kaplan, 1988). Power, a
concept recently explored by social psychologists, refers to the ability to influence others' outcomes
by virtue of someone's control over resources, often based on position in the hierarchy (Keltner,
Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003; Magee & Galinsky, 2008). Finally, it is useful to distinguish between
status hierarchies and
decision-making hierarchies, although these are often times conflated
particularly in humans (Van Vugt, 2006).
Leadership refers to a special position in the decision-
making hierarchy where individuals exercise disproportionate influence on group decision-making,
and can gain priority access to resources in return (Price & Van Vugt, 2014; Van Vugt, 2006).
An Evolutionary Psychology Perspective on Status
Given that a) status hierarchies are ubiquitous across observed human societies, both
modern and historical, b) status hierarchies are observed in non-human primates, as well as other
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 5
animals, and c) an individual's position in status hierarchies has consequences for access to sexual
partners and other fitness-relevant resources, humans likely possess evolved psychological
mechanisms that for status-striving and navigating status hierarchies. These mechanisms a) motivate
individuals to advance their positions in status hierarchies (
status improvement), b) convert
advantageous status positions into fitness benefits (
status capitalization), c) assess and monitor
others' positions in status hierarchies (
status assessment) and d) manage and cope with changes in
status positions in social hierarchies, both gains and losses (
status management ). These mechanisms
are instantiated as coordinated interactions between hormonal, cognitive, emotional, and behavioral
systems and they need not be consciously motivated.
An evolutionary psychology approach assumes that the psychological systems of status take
the shape of stimulus-response mechanisms that can be seen as conditional ("if-then") decision rules
that produce behaviors that were, on average, adaptive in the ancestral environment (Gigerenzer &
Goldstein, 1996; Tooby & Cosmides, 2005). A conditional rule such as: "Only challenge an individual's
status if the likely benefits outweigh the costs" enables individuals to achieve better pay-offs than a
decision rule to "challenge anyone's status." Multiple selection mechanisms might have shaped this
type of modular status psychology. Some evolutionary theories stress individual competition as the
basis for status differences, viewing hierarchy as the resultant of individuals pursuing their own
interests. Evolutionary biologist George Williams (1966) remarked: "The dominance-subordination
hierarchy…is not a functional organization. It is the statistical consequence of a compromise made by
each individual in its competition for food, mates, and other resources. Each compromise is adaptive
but not the statistical summation" (p. 218). Other evolutionary theorists stress the functionality of
status differences for both individuals and groups. One may be better off as a low status member in
a group with a stable hierarchy than a high status member in an unstable group (Caporael, 1997;
Ronay et al., 2012). Accordingly, social hierarchies in humans may be the product of selection
operating at multiple levels (e.g., group and individual; see Wilson, Van Vugt & O'Gorman, 2008;
compare with Pinker, this volume).
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 6
Game Theory and Status
Status striving can be conceptualized as a social strategy that has been selected for by virtue
of its role in fostering reproductive success. This can be illustrated by applying insights from
evolutionary game theory (Maynard Smith, 1982), which models social interactions as games in
which strategies compete with each other in a Darwinian fashion. Evolutionary game theory is like
economic game theory, except that the agents are genes, which embody strategies that are pitted
against alternative strategies. Strategies, and the genes that lead to their development within
individual organisms, spread through a population by virtue of the superior decision rules they
produce in fitness relevant situations, whereas inferior strategies are culled from the population.
We can model status interactions as different social strategies in a game of Chicken (Figure
1), which parallels the well-known Hawk-Dove game in evolutionary biology. The name "chicken"
stems from a game in which two car drivers drive towards each other on a collision course. One must
swerve or both may die in a crash; yet if one driver swerves and the other does not, the one who
swerved is called the chicken (coward). The principle of the game is therefore that while each player
prefers not to yield to the other, the worst possible outcome occurs when both players do not yield.
Status interactions have the feature of a Chicken game in which one can assume that players have
two available strategies, either to challenge for status (Dare) or avoid a status confrontation (Yield) --
these are akin to the hawk and dove strategies, respectively. A challenger always wins against an
avoider, and can therefore gain status. Pursuing a Dare strategy is thus effective when there are lots
of Yield types around who will accept your status. But as Darers become more common in a
population -- because they convert resources gained in status competitions into fitness -- interactions
between them will increase. These interactions result in negative payoffs for both parties (Figure 1).
In populations with many Darers, individuals who bow out of intense competition can thrive. The
Yielders might have to surrender resources to the Darers, but they avoid costly battles, and their
interactions with other Yielders are fruitful. Hence, there is a counter-surge of Yielder types in the
population. Under certain conditions, the population will reach a mixed equilibrium of Dare and Yield
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 7
strategies over time, at the point at which each strategy enjoys similar expected reproductive
success. This is a classic example of frequency-dependent selection (Buss, 2009; Maynard-Smith,
Animal research supports the principles of this status game. Consider orangutans, where
males differ greatly in size. The flanged males are physically large, and they attract females to their
territories by making loud vocal calls. The unflanged males are weaker and smaller, and they do not
hold a territory themselves. Their strategy is to avoid the big males, and to wait for the opportunity
to mate with an unguarded female (Harrison & Chivers, 2007). This explains why these two strategies
continue to co-exist in the male orangutan populations at particular frequencies. Similar alternative
mating strategies, reflecting the dare and yield tactics, have been observed in cuttlefish, salmon, and
beetles (Hunt & Simmons, 2001).
The Dare-Yield combinations are referred to as the game equilibria. Once interactions settle
into an equilibrium state, they are likely to remain there because neither player obtains a better
outcome by switching to a different strategy (this is called an ‘evolutionary stable strategy' (ESS) or in
economics and political science, a Nash equilibrium). This game thus selects for adaptations that
exploit equilibrium state of D-Y interactions, potentially giving rise to a stable status hierarchy. The
implications of the model for the formation of status hierarchies are multifold. First, the benefit of
Dare is higher in an interaction with a Yielder and a Yielder always defers to a Darer. Second, it is
better to Yield in interacting with a Darer (especially if the darer is likely to win the status
competition). Third, the combined pay-offs of the D-Y interaction are better than for pairs of either
D- or Y-types. Thus, groups composed of Darers and Yielders tend to have better gross payoffs than
homogenous groups. In contexts of intergroup competition, this dynamic might favor groups with a
mixed assortment of status strategies (for empirical evidence, see Ronay et al., 2012).
The Chicken game analysis offers a potentially valuable lens through which to think about
status adaptations and our evolved psychology of status. First, it shows why humans should strive to
improve their status, as this determines their differential access to resources (Frank, 1985). Second it
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 8
shows how groups can arrive at relatively stable status hierarchies (the game equilibria) instead of
facing constant challenges for status. Third, it provides an analytical framework for answering
questions that parallel some of the foundational issues within social and personality psychology – for
instance, how phenotypic qualities of the individual (a la the "person) interact with the situations
they find themselves in (cf. Kenrick & Funder, 1988; Lewin, 1946; Reis, 2008)? Individuals lacking the
phenotypic qualities to challenge for status, such as those lacking in physical strength or valuable
skills, should avoid the costs of status competition.
Fourth, these status strategies should be conditional. Individuals predisposed to challenge
for status should switch to a Yield strategy in situations in which they are likely to come up against a
more formidable opponent or an opponent that is more committed to compete. The latter explains
the home advantage effect documented in studies of animal behavior in which individuals that own
territories are likely to fight harder than the ones invading a territory. Fifth, depending upon relative
pay-offs of low or high status, we expect status confrontations to vary in intensity. The relative
benefits of a challenge, for instance, may be greater for men than for women, which could explain
the commonly observed sex differences in status striving, risk taking and dominance (Hogan, 2006).
Finally, the game offers insight into status challenges between groups, where one group lacking the
qualities to compete (e.g., through lack in numbers or resources) may yield to another group and a
stable between group hierarchy might emerge. The game analysis forms the foundation of social
psychological theories of intergroup processes, explaining why individuals identify more with high
status groups, and are motivated to make costs to improve their group standing (Tajfel & Turner,
This parsimonious game is naturally limited in a number of ways. It is agnostic regarding the
nature of the costs inflicted in status challenges (e.g., physical or psychological costs), and it says
nothing regarding the proximate mechanisms underlying status interactions -- how do individuals
signal their phenotypic qualities to each other and what qualities do they signal? Although we
assume that such signals have evolved to be honest, in actual status interactions signals may be
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 9
faked (e.g., lowering your voice pitch during a job talk). Finally, the game is agnostic about whether
status battles are based on dominance, prestige, or a combination (e.g., scientists and ultimate
fighters compete in very different ways for status).
Status Systems
Status and Hormones
Testosterone. Human and non-human animal studies suggest that hormones are one of the
proximate mechanisms that facilitate the emergence, development, and maintenance of status
hierarchies in groups. Levels of the androgen testosterone (T) relate to individuals' relative status in
both human and non-human samples (Archer, 1996; Ellis, 1995; Sapolsky, 1990), and not due only
because shared relationships with third variables such as age, sex, or size. After intra-sexual
competition – competition within one sex for access to mates -- victors on average experience an
increase in testosterone whereas losers experience a decrease in testosterone. This pattern has
been observed in direct physical competitions, in non-physical competitions, in experimental
competitions within the lab, and in natural competitions (e.g., wrestling; Gladue, Boechler, & McCaul,
1989; Mazur & Booth, 1998; Mazur, Booth, & Dabbs, 1992). Changes in testosterone also occur
vicariously and as a result of status competitions among groups. During the final match of the 1994
FIFA World Cup between Brazil and Italy, researchers found an increase in testosterone levels among
fans of the winning team and a decrease in testosterone levels among fans of the losing team
(Bernhardt, Dabbs, Fielden, & Lutter, 1998). The relationship between status and testosterone also
appears to be bidirectional, with changes in testosterone producing a change in position in the social
hierarchy. When biologists administered testosterone to low ranking cows, for example, the cows'
hierarchical positions increased; when testosterone was subsequently withdrawn, the cows' position
dropped (Bouissou, 1978).
Yet it is not quite clear how these testosterone changes convert into reproductive outcomes.
One possibility is that increasing T motivates individuals to adopt a dare strategy (cf. Archer, 1996).
Evidence linking testosterone and dominant/aggressive behaviors has been found among men in
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 10
highly intrasexually competitive prison populations and in non-prison populations (Mazur & Booth,
1998). Testosterone might also stimulate individuals to engage in prestige battles. After being
administered a small dose of testosterone, participants gave more money in an economic game,
compared to a control group receiving a placebo when giving money produced reputation benefits
(Eisenegger et al., 2010).
The nature of the relationship between testosterone and status can be illuminated by
considering the energetic tradeoffs that testosterone facilitates. Ellison (2003; Ellison & Ellison,
2009), for example, conceptualizes testosterone as regulating male reproductive effort. Higher
testosterone levels during development produce "masculine" traits such as a more prominent brow
ridge and larger jaw, a deeper pitched voice, and greater muscle mass. Such masculine traits are
useful in dominance competitions, including physical combat, as well as in prestige contests,
including mediation in conflicts (Von Rueden, 2014). Increases in testosterone following competition
victories can be interpreted as general increases in energy allocating to mating effort, then. If
testosterone also serves as an input into some of the psychological mechanisms governing mating
displays, then these increases in testosterone following success in intra-sexual competitions can lead
individuals to convert their victories into reproductive opportunities (
status capitalization).
Engaging in status competition can be costly, both in terms of the energetics associated with
testosterone production (e.g., Muehlenbein & Bribiescas, 2005) and in terms of the direct challenges
from conspecifics that increased testosterone leads to, fighting other challengers. Given these costs,
individuals who pursue an avoid strategy should be more wary of being placed in a high status
position in which they are frequently challenged. Josephs et al. (2006) provide some support for this
hypothesis. After rigging a competition, they observe that lower testosterone individuals placed into
a high status position (i.e., victors in a competition) and high testosterone individuals placed into a
low status position (i.e., losers in a competition) show patterns of relatively high arousal, with greater
heart rate, and worse performance on tests of cognitive performance. In addition, those individuals
who experienced increases in testosterone after losing the competition wanted to compete again
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 11
with the same individual; those individuals who decreased testosterone after losing wanted to avoid
another confrontation (Mehta & Josephs, 2006).
Cortisol and serotonin. Testosterone is not the only hormone that regulates positions in
status hierarchies. Indeed, cortisol also fluctuates in response to situations or events that might alter
positions in status hierarchies. Those situations that lead to transient increases in cortisol are often
subjectively experienced as "stressful." Like testosterone, cortisol functions to regulate the allocation
of energy to different physiological systems. Unlike testosterone, however, changes in cortisol
function to supply the organism with extra bursts of energy by extracting glucose from physiological
reserves that are lower priority in emergency situations (Ellison & Ellison, 2009). Adults and children
of low socio-economic status typically show higher cortisol levels than those of higher socio-
economic status, suggesting more frequent exposure to daily stressors (Kapuku, Treiber, & Davis,
2002; Marmot, 2004). Among married couples, the perception of the dominance of one's spouse
correlates with blood pressure reactivity during marital interactions (Brown, Smith, & Benjamin,
1998). Low-ranked managers have higher baseline levels of cortisol than higher ranked managers in
organizations (Sherman et al., 2012). Although the causal direction in these relationships is yet
unclear, it is consistent with Sapolsky's (1990) baboon studies showing that lowly ranked baboons
experience continuous elevated levels of cortisol. An interpretation of this effect is that cortisol
buffers against a low status position with an associated lack of resources (see Figure 1) by making
more energy reserves available.
Finally, high status has been linked to elevated levels of serotonin, a neurotransmitter,
primarily found in the central nervous system, which is thought to be related to feelings of happiness
and well-being. Primate studies have found that as individuals move up the social hierarchy of their
group, their serotonin levels increase (Sapolsky, 1990). Experiments with vervet monkeys show that
males with high social rank had almost twice as much serotonin in their blood as did the low-ranking
monkeys (Raleigh, McGuire, Brammer, & Yuwiler, 1984). A causal link between serotonin and status
was established when scientists administered citalopram (a serotonin drug) to 10 healthy volunteers.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 12
While taking the drug, these volunteers were rated as significantly more dominant by observers, and
they also increased their eye contact when interacting with strangers compared to a placebo group
(Tse & Bond, 2002). Not surprising, drugs to fight depression and anxiety in humans (e.g., Prozac)
work by increasing serotonin levels in the brain. Serotonin may well be an internal cue of one's status
position in a group.
Status and Physique
To the extent that position in status hierarchies and competitions for status (and, ultimately,
mates) favor physical size and strength, the highest quality individuals would be expected to be
bigger, at least for males (Kokko et al., [2002] note that physical size is partially influenced by
testosterone). Physical formidability offers obvious advantages during bouts of intra-sexual
competition, and it strongly predicts status in nonhumans; larger male baboons are ranked higher
than smaller male baboons (Johnson, 1987), and larger individuals are more likely to win dyadic
challenges in spiders (Taylor, Hasson, & Clark, 2001) and crayfish (Pavey & Fielder, 1996).
In human (males) there are two potential ways that physical size translates into higher
status. The first is through a series of physical dominance displays between intra-sexual competitors.
Larger males would, on average, win against smaller individuals, and larger individuals would rise to
the top of hierarchies. Naturally, this need not involve actual physical combat. An individual's stature
can be used as
information regarding that individual's likelihood of success in competition, and
confrontations are settled based on this information rather than actual combat, which would
decrease costs for all parties involved. In this sense, stature can be thought of as a cue to intra-sexual
competitive ability, and hence a critical piece of information in deciding who should be challenged
and who should be deferred to. Recent developmental studies show that even before the end of
their first year, human infants expect a physically larger individual to prevail over a physically smaller
individual in a dominance contest (Thomsen, Frankenhuis, Ingold-Smith, & Carey, 2011). Hence,
individuals of stature can simply avoid a status competition because they are not being challenged
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 13
This second is through physical size serving as a marker of someone's prestige.
Anthropological research suggests that physically stronger individuals may be better at procuring
resources for the group, defend the group against hostile outgroups, and settle intragroup disputes
(Von Rueden, 2014). Furthermore, there is evidence linking stature to health, intelligence, and
political influence, which are prestige indicators (Blaker & Van Vugt, 2014). Height is positively
associated with several variables associated with status, including income (Judge & Cable, 2004),
military rank (Mazur, Mazur, & Keating, 1984) and authority in the workplace (Gawley, Perks, &
Curtis, 2009). Furthermore, within businesses, individuals in managerial positions are on average
taller than individuals in non-managerial positions (Egolf & Corder, 1991; Murray & Schmitz, 2011).
Prestigious American science professors tend to be taller than the general public, and even the U.S.
presidential elections are won by the taller candidate at a rate greater than chance (McCann, 2001;
Stulp, Buunk, Verhulst, & Pollet, 2013). There is also evidence that being tall facilitates an individual's
upward social mobility. A study involving pairs of brothers and sisters found that the taller sibling was
on average better educated (Bielicki & Waliszko 1992). Different languages seem to reflect the
relationship between stature and status; in various cultures, traditional and modern, leaders and
other high status individuals are often referred to as "Big Men" (Van Vugt & Ahuja, 2011).
That people use height as a cue to others' placement in status hierarchies is demonstrated in
a recent experiment showing that taller male and female managers are perceived as better leaders
(Blaker et al., 2013). Whereas taller males were perceived as both more dominant, more intelligent,
and healthier, taller females were only seen as more intelligent. This suggests that stature might lead
to status benefits in both sexes, but that it does so via physical formidability more in men than in
There are other traits that might allow an individual to leverage physical formidability into
status. A handful of studies have reported that fat free muscle mass -- which is estimated by running
small electrical current through the body, and can be used as a proxy for physical strength -- is
positively correlated with wages for both males and females (Böckerman, Johansson, Kiiskinen, &
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 14
Heliövaara, 2010; Bozoyan & Wolbring, 2011). Men's physical strength also predicts their quickness
to anger and their likelihood of applying aggressive tactics to achieve their goals (Sell et al., 2009) --
thus being quicker to adopt a Dare strategy. Physical size at age three predicts aggressiveness and
disagreeableness at age 11, which suggests that strategies relating to physical strength are calibrated
early in life (Ishikawa, Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, & Lacasse, 2001). Physically stronger men also endorse
social norms that are beneficial to strong individuals. For example, Price et al. (2011) demonstrated
that physically stronger men have a stronger preference for social hierarchies and status inequalities.
Petersen et al. (2013) similarly found that upper body strength predicted men's endorsement of
resource redistribution policies that favored them: Poorer men's upper body strength predicted
stronger endorsement of wealth redistribution, whereas wealthier men's upper body strength
predicted weaker endorsement of wealth redistribution.
Other traits that might relate to success in intra-sexual competition also convey high status.
Facial masculinity -- which includes chin prominence, heaviness of brow ridges, and facial muscularity
-- predicts career development of military officers, with these traits being associated with higher
rankings within a military academy and more and quicker career promotions (Mueller & Mazur,
1996). The relationship between facial masculinity and success in hierarchical organizations might
result from both the tactics that more facially masculine individuals employ (dominance) and the
potential preferences for more facially dominant individuals as leaders (prestige). The latter is
supported by evidence that individuals vote for a more facially dominant leader, especially in the
context of war (Spisak et al., 2012).
Finally, physical attractiveness predicts a number of positive social outcomes afforded to
higher status individuals, such as having more dates and friends and making more money (Roszell,
Kennedy, & Grabb, 1989). In a study of college fraternities and sororities, more physically attractive
individuals were perceived as more prominent and occupied high status roles in these student
organizations more often (Anderson, John, Keltner, & Kring, 2001; Kalick, 1988). However, physical
attractiveness was more strongly predictive of social status in men than in women, suggesting that
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 15
attractive women may have an edge in competition for mates but not necessarily in challenges for
positions of leadership.
Verbal and Non-verbal Indicators of Status
In addition to leveraging physical capital into higher status, humans also employ various
behavioral tactics to convey their status (although whether they do this deliberately remains to be
seen). Consider a handshake. Shaking hands is a ubiquitous manner of introduction in the Western
world, especially between men who meet for the first time. Something as simple as grip strength
during a handshake might be an efficient manner of learning about another man's status. Socially
dominant and extraverted individuals have firmer handshakes (Stewart, Dustin, Barrick, & Darnold,
2008). High status individuals are also more likely to have an open, relaxed posture, show less
emotional expressivity in their face, and are less likely to laugh, especially in interacting with low
status individuals (Ketelaar et al., 2012). In a lab study, participants who viewed individuals engage in
subtly rude and norm violating behaviors rated these individuals as more decisive, strong, powerful,
and in control (Van Kleef et al., 2011). In a review of the literature on nonverbal behavioral
interactions, Argyle (1994) concludes that dominant individuals stand at full height with an expanded
chest, hold a firmer gaze, speak in a low-pitched voice, and gesture more.
A lower voice pitch can also provide information about an individual's status as it is related to
physical size and higher testosterone. Lower voice pitches are linked to status and occupational
success (Puts, Hodges, Cárdenas, & Gaulin, 2007). Indeed, in a recent study on CEO's of companies
registered on the American Stock Exchange showed that CEO's with lower the voices make more
money, with a 25% decrease in voice pitch being associated with a $187,000 increase in annual salary
(Mayew et al, 2013). Men also lower their voice pitch when they are addressing another man who is
lower in status, suggesting that voice pitch might be used to assert dominance in lieu of physical
competition (Puts, Gaulin, & Verdolini, 2006). Finally, verbal expressions may differ between high-
and low-status individuals. People are seen as more prominent and prestigious when they speak
more clearly, louder, confidently, and directly, whereas those who speak more softly and pepper
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 16
their comments with nervous giggles are seen as lower in status (Fiske, 2010). Moreover, high status
individuals often initiate conversations, shift discussions to their own areas of competence, and are
more likely to interrupt other speakers in the conversation (Godfrey, Jones, & Lord, 1986; Mast,
2002). Finally, displays of emotions convey status differences. Group members who express anger
are perceived to be of higher status than those who appear sad (Tiedens, 2001).
Status Changes and Emotions
Humans have likely evolved a suite of different emotional systems to negotiate positional
changes in status hierarchies (Tooby & Cosmides, 2008). When individuals emerge victorious in a
status competition (lower left cell of Figure 1) they experience happiness, elation and pride (Cheng,
Tracy, & Henrich, 2010; Tracy & Robins, 2007). In contrast, a status loss (upper right cell of Figure 1)
produces an increase in feelings of social anxiety, shame, rage, and depression (Gilbert, 1990).
Moreover, identical behaviors can elicit starkly different affective sensations depending on the status
consequences of the behavior.
Consider public speaking. As many readers of this chapter have experienced themselves,
giving a research presentation to a group of undergraduate students results in less anxiety than
giving the same talk to a mix of peers and more prestigious individuals at an international
conference. Presumably, this difference in anxiety reflects the different status consequences of a
good versus poor performance to the two groups, with poor performances in front of
undergraduates not affecting status as much as a poor performance in front of a group of scholarly
peers. Anxiety, or even the prospect of feeling anxious, might reflect the type of functional
forecasting and simulation discussed by Tooby and Cosmides (2008). That is, simulating the aversive
effects of actual status losses (i.e., experiencing anxiety) might lead individuals to either avoid
situations in which they are likely to lose status or invest extra effort into winning such competitions.
Similarly, people feel shame if they experience a loss in reputation, for example, after a moral
transgression (Giner-Sorolla & Espinosa, 2011; Haidt, 2003; Tracy & Robins, 2006). On the other
hand, when people lose a status competition that they feel they should have won, they may feel
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 17
rage, which might motivate them to seek a rematch or revenge. Finally, after experiencing a
prolonged loss of status (e.g., unemployment ), people may feel depressed, which motivates them to
temporarily avoid any status competition until they have gained enough resources to status
competitions. Depression symptoms indeed stop after people find a new job or start a new
relationship, at which point they might have the capital to reenter the fray of status competition
(Gilbert, 1990).
Other emotions could similarly guide behavior after status contests depending on the
outcome of the competition, their status, and the status of their competitor (Figure 1). After winning
a status contest, a high status individual might either experience pity or contempt for the low status
person, depending presumably on how the loser responds to his or her defeat, or the manner in
which the loser challenged the winner before the competition. In contrast, depending upon the
reactions of the winner, low status individuals might display admiration when they feel they have
legitimately lost the battle, and they might feel envy to motivate greater efforts during the next bout
Sex Differences in Status Striving
Like all mammals, men and women differ in their reproductive potential. Women invest
more heavily in offspring (e.g., via the time and energy invested in gestation and lactation) and the
number of years that they can conceive is constrained relative to men. Comparatively, men have
lower minimal obligate investment in offspring, they can sire more offspring (with another partner)
after a single act of conception, and they are reproductively viable for a longer period of their lives
(Trivers, 1972). As a result, the ceiling of reproductive output is higher for men, and men's
reproductive output tends to be more variable than female reproductive output (Bateman, 1948;
Brown, Laland, & Borgerhoff Mulder, 2009). These differences in minimal obligate investment form
the theoretical foundation for sex differences in mating strategies (e.g., Sexual Strategies Theory;
Buss & Schmitt, 1993) and myriad related sex differences ranging from aggression (Archer, 1996; Daly
& Wilson, 1988) to experiences of disgust toward unwanted sexual advances (Tybur, Lieberman,
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 18
Kurzban, & DeScioli, 2013). These sex differences also imply that there might be evolved sex
differences in psychological status systems.
Male reproductive output is more variable than female reproductive output, but which males
produce more offspring? As we discussed earlier, higher status males sire more offspring across
several species (Ellis, 1995). An extreme example comes from the northern elephant seals living off
the West coast of the United States and Mexico. Males compete for dominance before the breeding
season starts, and the winners get exclusive access to females, whereas the losers are excluded from
mating during the breeding season (Blaker & Van Vugt, 2014). Employing the game-theoretical model
presented earlier (Figure 1), this means that the relative pay-offs for Dare versus Yield will be greater
for male same sex interactions than for female same-sex interactions. The implication is that there is
a stronger incentive for males to compete for status than for females because of the larger
reproductive gains involved.
The relationship between male status and reproductive success also appears in humans. It
was particularly strong in early complex societies, such as the Aztec, Inca, and Mesopotamian
civilizations (Betzig, 1993). In these societies, access to women was strictly regulated, with higher
status men enjoying greater access to women than lower status men. In the more egalitarian hunter-
gatherer societies, the reproductive skew was arguably less pronounced, but the best hunters and
political leaders nevertheless enjoyed more sexual affairs. Indeed, among contemporary Tsimane – a
foraging people in the Bolivian lowlands in which pair bonding is normative – higher status men (both
dominant and prestigious men) have more extramarital sexual affairs than lower status men (Von
Rueden, Gurven, & Kaplan, 2011). The same applies to modern industrialized societies. Perusse
(1993) investigated the relationship between the position of male employees and their sexual
opportunities. The self-report data showed that employees with more senior positions had more
sexual liaisons. Young male members of street gangs are reported to have more sexual affairs and
greater status among their peers than non-gang members of the similar age (Palmer & Tilly, 1995).
American World War II soldiers who returned home as war heroes - recipients of the Congressional
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 19
Medal of Honor – had more children than other veterans who did not receive this award (Rusch,
Leunissen, & Van Vugt, 2014). This tendency for males to convert their high status into reproductive
success is common enough to be labeled with a specific term: the Bathsheba syndrome (Ludwig &
Longenecker, 1993).
Status, Mating, and Men's Psychology
Given the stakes of the outcomes of status competitions, men are expected to use more
costly tactics to advance their own status goals. These tactics often involve dominance displays (e.g.,
physical fights; Archer, 2009), and they are often used in response to otherwise trivial threats –
threats that only concern status rather than safety or tangible resources (Wilson & Daly, 1985).
Further, aggressive responses to status threats appear to be used more by men when other men –
other intrasexual competitors – are present to witness the outcome of the competition (Griskevicius,
Tybur et al., 2009). Additionally, men gain status by participating in coalitional fights against other
men of rival groups. Men contribute more to their groups in settings of intergroup competition than
in the absence of intergroup competition, whereas women do not (Van Vugt et al., 2007). Men also
report more aggressive intergroup encounters than women, and they are more likely to support and
participate in between group violence (the male warrior effect; Van Vugt et al., 2007). Finally, men
score higher on Social Dominance Orientation, which measures the extent to which people prefer
status differences and unequal resource access between groups in society (Pratto, Sidanius et al.,
1994). The direction of this sex difference is invariant across cultures, even appearing in relatively
egalitarian societies such as Sweden and the Netherlands. Intergroup aggression may be a preferred
tactic for especially low status males to elevate their status via combat, and therefore increase their
access to resources (Chagnon, 1990; McDonald, Navarrete & Van Vugt, 2012; Navarrete et al., 2010).
Men may also apply prestige tactics to attract sexual mates. When groups of male
participants were playing a public good game in a laboratory study and were being watched by an
attractive woman, they donated more to the group fund than when there was no audience or when
man audience (Van Vugt & Iredale, 2010). Additionally, men donate more to street beggars when in
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 20
the presence of female company rather than male company or alone (Iredale et al., 2008). Finally
when men and women were primed with romantic motives and were then asked about their helping
decisions, men endorsed engaging in heroic, status-enhancing forms of helping (e.g., jumping into
water to help someone who is drowning; Griskevicius, Tybur et al., 2007). In contrast, women
endorsed more conventional, low risk helping (e.g., volunteer work) after a romantic prime. In a
virtual environment, men cross a scary rope bridge faster when observed by female bystanders
compared to male bystanders (Frankenhuis, Dotsch, Karremans, & Wigboldus, 2010). Finally, men
who are more committed to their current partner self-reportedly take fewer risks than men who are
less committed to their partner (Frankenhuis & Karremans, 2012).
Status and Women's Psychology
If men's status conveys information regarding benefits to women (e.g., as mates), then
selection might favor a female mating psychology that finds status attractive. In Buss's (1989)
landmark cross-cultural study on mate preferences, females across cultures valued status relevant
traits in a romantic partner (e.g., earning capacity, ambition) more than men. In lab studies, females
express greater sexual interest in dominant men, but men do not express greater sexual interest in
dominant women (Sadalla, Kenrick, & Vershure, 1987). When asked to "build a mate" using a limited
budget, women prioritize status and resources in constructing their mate more than men (Li et al.,
2002). Men are not blind to this preference; they are more likely to advertise their status and
resources on personal romantic advertisements relative to women (Pawlowski & Dunbar, 1999).
Women's mate preferences for status could reflect both preferences for the direct (i.e.,
protection and resources) and indirect (i.e., heritable quality) benefits that high status men might
possess. The former is certainly true, with women prioritizing social status and resources more in
long-term mates relative to short-term sexual partners (Li & Kenrick, 2006). The latter also appears to
be true. For example, women's preferences for the type of dominant facial structures described
previously are highest at peak fecundability (Johnston et al., 2001; Penton-Voak & Perrett, 2000), as
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 21
are preferences for men's intrasexually competitive behaviors (Gangestad et al., 2004). Further,
preferences for such traits are often observed only when female participants judge attractiveness as
a short-term sexual partner (see Thornhill and Gangestad, 2008, for a theoretical overview; see
Gildersleeve et al., 2014, for a recent meta-analysis). The data suggest a clear picture – women prefer
status, resources, and morphological and social dominance displays in men, both for the direct and
indirect benefits that these traits afford.
A final note on sex differences is that men and women might follow different tactics to
acquire status. In one study, men and women rated the social desirability of many different
dominance acts (Buss, 1981). The main conclusion is that men are more accepting of egoistic
dominant acts such as ""Managing to get one's way" or "Complaining about having to do a favor for
someone." Women were more accepting of more prosocial, prestigious acts such as "Being active in
many community and campus activities" or "taking charge of things at the committee meeting."
The Evolutionary Psychology of Leadership
The final section focuses on one particular hierarchical position in groups: leadership. Leaders
enjoy considerable prestige in most human societies, and the associated benefits should make
leadership positions particularly attractive (Van Vugt, 2006). The dynamics of leadership are
complicated, though, and not all individuals seek out leadership positions, and not all leaders are
afforded similar status. Evolutionary biologists have had an enduring interest in leadership, and there
is a growing literature on the subject dedicated to unravelling some of these complications (e.g., King
et al., 2009). Although leadership has been and continues to be a hugely popular theme in the social
sciences, this literature has traditionally not addressed fundamental questions about leadership,
such as why individuals allow leaders to emerge, why individuals would incur the costs of taking up
leadership roles, etc. Following Price and van Vugt (2014), we view the topic as a reciprocal exchange
between leaders and followers where status and prestige benefits accrue to individual leaders as
they provide public goods to followers.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 22
In human societies, leaders are often highly respected, liked, and admired, with Nelson
Mandela and Mohandas Gandhi serving as peak examples. This stands in stark contrast with the
highest status individual in non-human primate groups such as gorillas and chimpanzees where
dominant males (alphas) appear to be feared and, at the risk of anthropomorphizing internal states,
loathed by lower ranking individuals (King et al., 2009; Van Vugt, 2006). Also leaders in human groups
cannot monopolize resources to the same degree as the alpha in non-human primates. This raises a
critical question: if human leaders do not dominate access to resources as non-human primate
alphas do, but they still invest disproportionate resources into their groups, then why would they
seek out and accept such positions? The service-for-prestige theory (Price & Van Vugt, 2014)
contributes to solving this puzzle.
Human leadership is characterized by voluntary, reciprocal arrangements between leaders
and followers (cf. Trivers, 1971). In this reciprocal dynamic, leaders trade their expertise, skills,
education, personal risks, and time in exchange for prestige offered by followers. This dynamic works
best for followers when power differentials between leaders and followers are small, thus when
leaders have limited opportunities to use their position to exploit followers. Situations in which
power differentials between leaders and followers are large tend to produce status hierarchies based
on dominance rather than prestige (Van Vugt et al., 2008). Furthermore, giving prestige benefits to
leaders poses a collective action problem among followers as it is cheaper to profit from leaders'
group contributions while not deferring to them. To the extent that groups are better at solving this
free-rider problem this will facilitate good leadership.
Several observations support the service-for-prestige idea. First, individuals who achieve
leadership positions in foraging societies often do so via public displays of expertise (e.g., hunting,
political influence). Among Amazonian Shuar, individuals who are perceived as providing the most
valuable service to their social groups are preferred as group leaders and receive more esteem from
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 23
others in the group (Price, 2003). Lab experiments similarly show that participants who demonstrate
a willingness -- and ability -- to provide benefits to the group receive more status (Anderson &
Kilduff, 2009; Hardy & Van Vugt, 2006; Willer, 2009). Finally, the status benefits associated with
taking on leadership roles are often converted into reproductive success (Von Rueden, 2014).
Second, traits valued in leaders in Western societies include intelligence, vision, persistence,
communication skills, persuasion, fairness and ethical decision making (Judge, Colbert, & Ilies, 2004).
These are also among the leader traits most valued by followers within traditional societies (Tooby,
Cosmides, & Price, 2006; Von Rueden, 2014). This suggests that there is cross-cultural consistency in
what followers expect from leaders (Den Hartog et al., 1999). Communication and oratory skills
facilitate social coordination, higher intelligence would conceivably relate to better decision-making,
and fairness would guard against exploitation of followers (Van Vugt et al., 2008).
A third observation in line with service for prestige is based upon considerations of the costs
that leaders can impose upon followers (Padilla, Hogan, & Kaiser, 2007). As leaders accumulate
power over time, their positions in status hierarchies can transition from prestige based to
dominance based as they can start to monopolize resources. In mutually beneficial reciprocal
relationships between partners of relatively equal power, individuals are partially motivated to treat
their partners well, because poor treatment can lead the partner to exit the relationship and devote
resources elsewhere (Van Vugt et al., 2004). As individuals become more dependent on leaders for
organizing collective action and the distribution of resources, they become less able to leave the
relationship, hence leaders' incentives to treat their followers fairly decreases. With increases in
group size and population density, and, perhaps most importantly, decreases in population mobility,
leaders can accumulate power more easily, and leader-follower relationships can transition toward
being dominant and exploitative. This is especially the case when levelling mechanisms that might
rein in power abuses such as criticism, salary caps, and replacement of leaders are absent (Boehm,
1999; Van Vugt & Ahuja, 2011). The consequences of such shifts in power are underscored by the
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 24
psychological literature, which suggests that increases in power decreases empathy (Galinsky et al.,
2006) and increases abuse (Kipnis, 1972). Furthermore, anecdotal reports suggest that among the
higher echelon leaders in politics and business, there is a preponderance of males with dark triad
personalities – a combination of Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy (Babiak & Hare,
2006; Ludwig, 2002).
Conclusions
By analyzing status from an evolutionary perspective, this chapter attempts to make various
contributions to the literature. First, it distinguishes status from a number of related constructs that
are often used interchangeably in the literature, including power, dominance, prestige, and
leadership. Second, it differentiates conceptually status hierarchies from decision-making
hierarchies. Third, it contributes to a foundation for better understanding status via an adaptationist
lens by considering the origins, functions, development, and psychological mechanisms underlying
status striving in humans, partially by viewing status competitions via a simple game theory model. In
doing so, it highlights distinctions between evolved psychological systems for signaling status,
assessing status, managing status change, and converting status into reproductive opportunities.
Fourth, it highlights the role that hormones, physical attributes, and emotions can play in facilitating
individuals' positions in status hierarchies.
Although we have endeavored to cover a wide range of topics on status, this chapter has also
been limited in scope, partially based on outstanding questions that have yet to be solved. Notably,
we have not examined the causes of the variability in status hierarchies between human societies,
and between humans and nonhumans. Yet it is clear that some societies (and species) are more
hierarchical than others. Theorists have asserted that hierarchies are attenuated when (a) resources
are more difficult to monopolize, (b) sharing resources is essential for survival, (c) individuals can
easily leave groups, and (d) form coalitions to overthrow a dominant. Further work should
investigate the importance of these levelling mechanisms in the formation of status hierarchies
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 25
(Cashdan, 1983; Plavcan, van Schaik & Kappeler, 1995). Additionally, more work can be done on the
game. Our treatment of the Chicken game implied two different phenotypes. Yet there is naturally
great variability in human personality (Buss, 2009; Nettle, 2006), which might partially reflect
frequency-dependent selection on dispositions analogous to the two status strategies. Future
research could test the degree to which heritable personality variation relates to behavior in status
competitions (cf. Verweij et al., 2012). Third, our treatment of female status striving has been
limited (sew Campbell, this volume, for an in-depth treatment of female status). The literature
describing female intrasexual competition is building (e.g., Beneson, 2013; Campbell, 1999, 2013;
Grant & France, 2001; Hess & Hagen, 2006; Pusey, 1997), but more work is required to elucidate the
effects of intra-sexual competition on losers and winners in female-female contests, and, ultimately,
the fitness benefits that women procure by moving up female-specific status hierarchies. Other lines
of research might further investigate status striving tendencies throughout the lifespan of humans,
and the different tactics to gain status by older and younger individuals (Wilson & Daly, 1985).
Answering questions such as these can shed light onto the psychology of human status, and can help
us understand questions regarding the evolutionary foundations of status hierarchy.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 26
References
Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (2001). Who attains social status? Effects of
personality and physical attractiveness in social groups.
Journal of personality and social
psychology,
81(1), 116.
Anderson, C., & Kilduff, G. J. (2009). Why do dominant personalities attain influence in face-to-face
groups? The competence-signaling effects of trait dominance.
Journal of personality and social
psychology,
96(2), 491.
Archer, J. (1996). Sex differences in social behavior: are the social role and evolutionary explanations
compatible?.
American Psychologist,
51(9), 909.
Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression?.
Behavioral and
Brain Sciences,
32(3-4), 249-266.
Argyle, M. (1994).
The psychology of interpersonal behaviour. Penguin UK.
Babiak, P., & Hare, R. D. (2006).
Snakes in suits: When psychopaths go to work. Regan Books/Harper
Collins Publishers.
Baker, S. (2010).
Ancient Rome: The rise and fall of an empire. London, UK: BBC Digital.
Bateman, A. J. (1948). Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila.
Heredity,
2(Pt. 3), 349-368.
Benenson, J. F. (2013). The development of human female competition: allies and
adversaries.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
368(1631).
Bernhardt, P. C., Dabbs Jr, J. M., Fielden, J. A., & Lutter, C. D. (1998). Testosterone changes during
vicarious experiences of winning and losing among fans at sporting events.
Physiology &
Behavior,
65(1), 59-62.
Betzig, L. (1993). Sex, succession, and stratification in the first six civilizations: How powerful men
reproduced, passed power on to their sons, and used power to defend their wealth, women,
and children. In L. Ellis (Ed.),
Social stratification and socioeconomic inequality (Vol.1, pp. 37–
74). Westport, CT: Praeger
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 27
Bielicki, T., & Waliszko, H. (1992). Stature, upward social mobility and the nature of statural
differences between social classes.
Annals of human biology,
19(6), 589-593.
Blaker, N. M., Rompa, I., Dessing, I. H., Vriend, A. F., Herschberg, C., & van Vugt, M. (2013). The
height leadership advantage in men and women: Testing evolutionary psychology predictions
about the perceptions of tall leaders.
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations,
16(1), 17-27.
Blaker, N. M., & van Vugt, M. (2014). The status-size hypothesis: Physical correlates of social status.
In J. Cheng, J. Tracy, & C. Anderson (Eds.),
The psychology of status. In press.
Böckerman, P., Johansson, E., Kiiskinen, U., & Heliövaara, M. (2010). The relationship between
physical work and the height premium: Finnish evidence.
Economics & Human Biology,
8(3),
Boehm, C. (1999).
Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard: Harvard
University Press.
Bouissou, M. F. (1978). Effects of injections of testosterone propionate on dominance relationships in
a group of cows.
Hormones and Behavior, 11, 388-400
Brown, D. E. (1991).
Human universals. Temple University Press.
Brown, G. R., Laland, K. N., & Mulder, M. B. (2009). Bateman's principles and human sex roles.
Trends
in Ecology & Evolution,
24(6), 297-304.
Brown, P. C., Smith, T. W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1998). Perceptions of spouse dominance predict blood
pressure reactivity during marital interactions.
Annals of Behavioral Medicine,
20(4), 286-293.
Buss, D. M. (1981). Sex differences in the evaluation and performance of dominant acts.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 147-154.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37
cultures.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
12(1), 1-49.
Buss, D. M. (2009). How can evolutionary psychology successfully explain personality and individual
differences?
Perspectives on Psychological Science,
4(4), 359-366.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 28
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human
mating.
Psychological Review,
100(2), 204.
Campbell, A. (1999). Staying alive: Evolution, culture, and women's intrasexual aggression.
Behavioral
and Brain Sciences,
22(02), 203-214.
Campbell, A. (2013). The evolutionary psychology of women's aggression .
Philosophical Transactions
of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
368(1631).
Caporael, L. R. (1997). The evolution of truly social cognition: The core configurations
model.
Personality and Social Psychology Review,
1(4), 276-298.
Cashdan (1983). Territoriality among human foragers: Ecological models and an application to
four Bushman groups.
Current Anthropology, 24, 47-66.
Chagnon, N. A. (1990). Reproductive and somatic conflicts of interest in the genesis of violence and
warfare among tribesmen.
The anthropology of war, 77-104.
Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010). Pride, personality, and the evolutionary foundations of
human social status.
Evolution and Human Behavior,
31, 334-347.
Cummins, D. (2005). Dominance, status, and social hierarchies. In D.M. Buss (Ed.),
Handbook of
evolutionary psychology (pp. 676-697). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Den Hartog, D. et al. (1999). Culture specific and cross culturally generalizable implicit leadership
theories: Are attributes of charismatic/transformational leadership universally endorsed?
Leadership Quarterly, 10, 219-256.
Egolf, D. B., & Corder, L. E. (1991). Height differences of low and high job status, female and male
corporate employees.
Sex Roles,
24(5-6), 365-373.
Eisenegger, C., Naef, M., Snozzi, R., Heinrichs, M., & Fehr, E. (2010). Prejudice and truth about the
effect of testosterone on human bargaining behavior.
Nature, 463, 356-359.
Ellis, L. (1995). Dominance and reproductive success among non-human animals.
Ethology and
Sociobiology, 16, 257-333.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 29
Ellison, P. T. (2003). Energetics and reproductive effort.
American Journal of Human Biology,
15(3),
Ellison, P. T., & Ellison, P. T. (2009).
On fertile ground: A natural history of human reproduction.
Harvard University Press.
Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: framework for a unified theory of social
relations.
Psychological Review,
99(4), 689.
Fiske, S. T. (2010). Interpersonal stratification: Status, power, and subordination.
Handbook of social
psychology.
Frank, R. H. (1985).
Choosing the right pond: Human behavior and the quest for status. Oxford
University Press.
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Karremans, J. C. (2012). Uncommitted men match their risk taking to female
preferences, while committed men do the opposite.
Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 48, 428-431.
Frankenhuis, W. E., Dotsch, R., Karremans, J. C., & Wigboldus, D. H. J. (2010). Male physical risk taking
in a virtual environment.
Journal of Evolutionary Psychology,
8, 75-86.
Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power and perspectives not
taken.
Psychological Science,
17(12), 1068-1074.
Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Christensen, P. N. (2004).
Women's preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual
cycle.
Psychological Science,
15(3), 203-207.
Gawley, T., Perks, T., & Curtis, J. (2009). Height, gender, and authority status at work: analyses for a
national sample of Canadian workers.
Sex Roles,
60(3-4), 208-222.
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded
rationality.
Psychological Review,
103(4), 650.
Gilbert, P. (1990). Changes: Rank status, and mood. In S. Fischer & C. L. Cooper (Eds.).
On the move:
The psychology of change and transition (pp. 33-52). New York: Wiley.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 30
Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. R. (in press). Do Women's Mate Preferences Change
Across the Ovulatory Cycle? A Meta-Analytic Review.
Psychological Bulletin.
Gillet, J., Cartwright, E., & Van Vugt, M. (2011). Selfish or servant leadership? Evolutionary
predictions on leadership personalities in coordination games.
Personality and Individual
Differences, 51, 231-236
Giner-Sorolla, R., & Espinosa, P. (2011). Social cuing of guilt by anger and of shame by
disgust.
Psychological Science,
22(1), 49-53.
Gladue, B. A., Boechler, M., & McCaul, K. D. (1989). Hormonal response to competition in human
males.
Aggressive Behavior,
15(6), 409-422.
Godfrey, D. K., Jones, E. E., & Lord, C. G. (1986). Self-promotion is not ingratiating.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,
50(1), 106.
Grant, V. J., & France, J. T. (2001). Dominance and testosterone in women.
Biological psychology,
58(1), 41-47.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Gangestad, S. W., Perea, E. F., Shapiro, J. R., & Kenrick, D. T. (2009).
Aggress to impress: hostility as an evolved context-dependent strategy.
Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,
96(5), 980.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Sundie, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Miller, G. F., & Kenrick, D. T. (2007). Blatant
benevolence and conspicuous consumption: when romantic motives elicit strategic costly
signals.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
93(1), 85.
Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions.
Handbook of affective sciences, 852-870.
Hardy, C. L., & Van Vugt, M. (2006). Nice guys finish first: The competitive altruism
ypothesis.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
32(10), 1402-1413.
Harrison, M. E., & Chivers, D. J. (2007). The orang-utan mating system and the unflanged male: A
product of increased food stress during the late Miocene and Pliocene?
Journal of Human
Evolution,
52(3), 275-293.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 31
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a
mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission.
Evolution and human
behavior,
22(3), 165-196.
Hess, N. H., & Hagen, E. H. (2006). Sex differences in indirect aggression: Psychological evidence from
young adults.
Evolution and Human Behavior,
27(3), 231-245.
Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1988). Tradeoffs in male and female reproductive strategies among the Ache:
Part 1.
Human reproductive behaviour: a Darwinian perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press. p, 277-289.
Hunt, J., & Simmons, L. W. (2001). Status-dependent selection in the dimorphic beetle Onthophagus
taurus.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences,
268(1484),
Iredale, W., Van Vugt, M., & Dunbar, R. (2008). Showing off in humans: Male generosity as a mating
signal.
Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 386–392.
Ishikawa, S. S., Raine, A., Lencz, T., Bihrle, S., & Lacasse, L. (2001). Autonomic stress reactivity and
executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psychopaths from the
community.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
110(3), 423.
Johnson, J. A. (1987). Dominance rank in juvenile olive baboons, Papio anubis: the influence of
gender, size, maternal rank and orphaning.
Animal Behaviour,
35, 1694-1708.
Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B., & Grammer, K. (2001). Male facial attractiveness:
Evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design.
Evolution and Human Behavior,
22(4), 251-
Josephs, R. A., Sellers, J. G., Newman, M. L., & Mehta, P. H. (2006). The mismatch effect: when
testosterone and status are at odds.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
90(6), 999.
Judge, T. A., & Cable, D. M. (2004). The effect of physical height on workplace success and income:
preliminary test of a theoretical model.
Journal of Applied Psychology,
89(3), 428.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 32
Judge, T. A., Colbert, A. E., & Ilies, R. (2004). Intelligence and leadership: a quantitative review and
test of theoretical propositions.
Journal of Applied Psychology,
89(3), 542.
Kalick, S. M. (1988). Physical attractiveness as a status cue.
Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology,
24(6), 469-489.
Kapuku, G. K., Treiber, F. A., & Davis, H. C. (2002). Relationships among socioeconomic status, stress
induced changes in cortisol, and blood pressure in African American males.
Annals of
Behavioral Medicine,
24(4), 320-325.
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition.
Psychological
Review,
110(2), 265-284.
Kenrick, D. T., & Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: Lessons from the person-situation
debate.
American Psychologist,
43(1), 23.
Ketelaar, T., Koenig, B. L., Gambacorta, D., Dolgov, I., Hor, D., Zarzosa, J., . & Wells, L. (2012). Smiles
as signals of lower status in football players and fashion models: Evidence that smiles are
associated with lower dominance and lower prestige.
Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3), 371.
King, A. J., Johnson, D. D., & Van Vugt, M. (2009). The origins and evolution of leadership.
Current
Biology,
19(19), R911-R916.
Kipnis, D. (1972). Does power corrupt?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 33-41.
Kokko, H., Brooks, R., McNamara, J. M., & Houston, A. I. (2002). The sexual selection
continuum.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological
Sciences,
269(1498), 1331-1340.
Lewin, K. (1946). Action research and minority problems.
Journal of Social Issues,
2(4), 34-46.
Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. (2002). The necessities and luxuries of mate
preferences: testing the tradeoffs.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
82(6), 947.
Li, N. P., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Sex similarities and differences in preferences for short-term mates:
what, whether, and why.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
90(3), 468.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 33
Ludwig, A. (2002).
King of the mountain: The nature of political leadership. Kentucky: Kentucky
University Press.
Ludwig, D. C., & Longenecker, C. O. (1993). The Bathsheba syndrome: the ethical failure of successful
leaders.
Journal of Business Ethics,
12(4), 265-273.
McDonald, M. M., Navarrete, C. D., & Van Vugt, M. (2012). Evolution and the Psychology of
Intergroup Conflict: The Male Warrior Hypothesis.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society-Biological Sciences, 367, 670-679
Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). 8 Social Hierarchy: The Self‐Reinforcing Nature of Power and
Status.
The Academy of Management Annals,
2(1), 351-398.
Marmot, M. (2004).
Status syndrome. London, UK: Wiley.
Mast, M. S. (2002). Dominance as expressed and inferred through speaking time.
Human
Communication Research,
28(3), 420-450.
Mayew, W. J., Parsons, C., & Venkatachalam, M. (2013). Voice pitch and the labor market success of
male chief executive officers.
Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 243-248.
Maynard Smith, J. (1982).
Evolution and the Theory of Games. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men.
Behavioral and Brain
Sciences,
21(3), 353-363.
Mazur, A., Booth, A., & Dabbs Jr, J. M. (1992). Testosterone and chess competition.
Social Psychology
Quarterly, 70-77.
Mazur, A., Mazur, J., & Keating, C. (1984). Military rank attainment of a West Point class: Effects of
cadets' physical features.
American Journal of Sociology, 125-150.
McCann, S. J. (2001). Height, societal threat, and the victory margin in presidential elections (1824-
1992).
Psychological Reports,
88(3), 741-742.
Mehta, P. H., & Josephs, R. A. (2006). Testosterone change after losing predicts the decision to
compete again.
Hormones and Behavior,
50(5), 684-692.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 34
Muehlenbein, M. P., & Bribiescas, R. G. (2005). Testosterone‐mediated immune functions and male
life histories.
American Journal of Human Biology,
17(5), 527-558.
Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (1996). Facial dominance of West Point cadets as a predictor of later military
rank.
Social Forces,
74(3), 823-850.
Murray, G. R., & Schmitz, D. J. (2011). Caveman politics: Evolutionary leadership preferences and
physical stature.
Social Science Quarterly, 92, 1215-1235.
Navarrete, C. D., McDonald, M. M., Molina, L. E., & Sidanius, J. (2010). Prejudice at the nexus of race
and gender: an outgroup male target hypothesis.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,
98(6), 933.
Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation in humans and other animals.
American
Psychologist,
61(6), 622.
Padilla, A., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2007). The toxic triangle: Destructive leaders, susceptible
followers, and conducive environments.
The Leadership Quarterly,
18(3), 176-194.
Palmer, C. T., & Tilley, C. F. (1995). Sexual access to females as a motivation for joining gangs: An
evolutionary approach.
Journal of Sex Research,
32(3), 213-217.
Pavey, C. R., & Fielder, D. R. (1996). The influence of size differntial on agonistic behaviour in the
freshwater crayfish, Cherax cuspidatus (Decapoda: Parastacidae).
Journal of Zoology,
238(3),
Pawlowski, B., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (1999). Impact of market value on human mate choice decisions.
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 266, 281 – 285.
Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2000). Female preference for male faces changes cyclically:
Further evidence.
Evolution and Human Behavior,
21(1), 39-48.
Perusse, D. (1993). Cultural and reproductive success in industrial societies: Testing the relationship
at the proximate and ultimate levels.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(02), 267-283.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 35
Petersen, M. B., Sznycer, D., Sell, A., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). The ancestral logic of politics:
Upper body strength regulates men's assertion of self-interest over economic
redistribution.
Psychological Science.
Plavcan, J. M., van Schaik, C. P., & Kappeler, P. M. (1995). Competition, coalitions and canine size in
primates.
Journal of Human Evolution,
28(3), 245-276. Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M.,
& Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and
political attitudes.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
67(4), 741.
Price M. E. (2003). Pro-community altruism and social status in a Shuar village.
Human
Nature, 14, 191-208.
Price, M. E. ,Kang, J., Dunn,J. ,Hopkins, S. (2011). Muscularity and attractivenesss as predictors of
human egalitarianism.
Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 636–640.
Price, M. E., & van Vugt, M. (2014). The service for prestige theory of leadership. In S. Colarelli & R.
Arvey,
Handbook of the Biology of Organizational Behavior. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Pusey, A., Williams, J., & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive
success of female chimpanzees.
Science,
277(5327), 828-831.
Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in
human voice pitch.
Evolution and Human Behavior,
27(4), 283-296.
Puts, D. A., Hodges, C. R., Cárdenas, R. A., & Gaulin, S. J. (2007). Men's voices as dominance signals:
vocal fundamental and formant frequencies influence dominance attributions among
men.
Evolution and Human Behavior,
28(5), 340-344.
Raleigh, M. J., McGuire, M. T., Brammer, G. L., & Yuwiler, A. (1984). Social and environmental
influences on blood serotonin concentrations in monkeys.
Archives of General
Psychiatry,
41(4), 405.
Reis, H. T. (2008). Reinvigorating the concept of situation in social psychology.
Personality and Social
Psychology Review,
12(4), 311-329.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 36
Ronay, R., Greenaway, K., Anicich, E. M., & Galinsky, A. D. (2012). The path to glory is paved with
hierarchy when hierarchical differentiation increases group effectiveness.
Psychological
Science,
23(6), 669-677.
Roszell, P., Kennedy, D., & Grabb, E. (1989). Physical attractiveness and income attainment among
Canadians.
The Journal of Psychology,
123(6), 547-559.
Rusch, H., Leunissen, J., & van Vugt, M. (2014).
Sexual attraction to male warriors. Unpublished
manuscript: VU University
Sadalla, E. K., Kenrick, D. T., & Vershure, B. (1987). Dominance and heterosexual attraction.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,
52(4), 730.
Sapolsky, R. M. (1990). Adrenocortical function, social rank, and personality among wild baboons.
Biological Psychiatry, 28, 862-878.
Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009). Formidability and the logic of human anger.
Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, 106(35), 15073-15078.
Sherman, G. D., Lee, J. J., Cuddy, A. J. C., Renshon, J., Oveis, C., Gross, J., & Lerner, J. (2012).
Leadership is associated with lower levels of stress.
PNAS, 109, 17903-17907
Spisak, B. R., Dekker, P. H., Krüger, M., & van Vugt, M. (2012). Warriors and peacekeepers: Testing a
biosocial implicit leadership hypothesis of intergroup relations using masculine and feminine
faces.
PLOS-One,
7(1), e30399.
Stewart, G. L., Dustin, S. L., Barrick, M. R., & Darnold, T. C. (2008). Exploring the handshake in
employment interviews.
Journal of Applied Psychology,
93(5), 1139.
Stulp, G., Buunk, A.P., Verhulst, S., & Pollet, T.V. (2013). Tall claims? Sense and nonsense about the
importance of height of US presidents.
The Leadership Quarterly, 24(1), 159-171.
Tajfel, H. & Turner, J. C. (2004). The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior. In J. Jost, and
J. Sidanius (Eds), Political psychology: Key readings. Key readings in social psychology.
(pp. 276-293). New York, NY, US: Psychology Press, xiii, 497 pp.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 37
Taylor, P. W., Hasson, O., & Clark, D. L. (2001). Initiation and resolution of jumping spider contests:
roles for size, proximity, and early detection of rivals.
Behavioral Ecology and
Sociobiology,
50(5), 403-413.
Thomsen, L., Frankenhuis, W. E., Ingold-Smith, M., & Carey, S. (2011). Big and mighty: Preverbal
infants mentally represent social dominance.
Science,
331(6016), 477-480.
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008).
The evolutionary biology of human female sexuality. Oxford
University Press.
Tiedens, L. Z. (2001). Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: the effect of negative
emotion expressions on social status conferral.
Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,
80(1), 86.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In D.M. Buss
(Ed.),
The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 5–67). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship
to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones & L. F. Barrett (Eds.),
Handbook of Emotions, 3rd Ed. (pp. 114-137.) NY: Guilford.
Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., & Price, M. E. (2006). Cognitive adaptations for n‐person exchange: the
evolutionary roots of organizational behavior.
Managerial and Decision Economics,
27(2‐3),
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2006). Appraisal antecedents of shame and guilt: Support for a
theoretical model.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
32(10), 1339-1351.
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). Emerging insights into the nature and function of pride.
Current
Directions in Psychological Science,
16(3), 147-150.
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism.
Quarterly Review of Biology, 35-57.
Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.),
Sexual selection and
the descent of man, 1871–1971 (pp.136 –179). Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 38
Tse, W. S., & Bond, A. L. (2002). Serotonergic intervention affects both social dominance and
affiliative behavior.
Psychopharmacology, 161, 324-330.
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and
structure.
Psychological Review, 120, 65–84.
Van Kleef, G. A., Homan, A. C., Finkenauer, C., Gündemir, S., & Stamkou, E. (2011). Breaking the Rules
to Rise to Power How Norm Violators Gain Power in the Eyes of Others.
Social Psychological
and Personality Science,
2(5), 500-507.
Van Vugt, M. (2006). Evolutionary origins of leadership and followership.
Personality and Social
Psychology Review,
10, 354-371.
Van Vugt, M., & Ahuja, A. (2011).
Naturally Selected: Why Some People Lead, Why Others Follow, and
Why It Matters. HarperCollins.
Van Vugt, M., De Cremer, D., & Janssen, D. (2007). Gender differences in cooperation and
competition: The male warrior hypothesis.
Psychological Science, 18, 19-23.
Van Vugt, M., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2008). Leadership, followership, and evolution: some lessons
from the past.
American Psychologist,
63(3), 182.
Van Vugt, M., & Iredale, W. (2012). Men behaving nicely: Public goods as peacock tails.
British Journal
of Psychology.
Van Vugt, M., Jepson, S., Hart, C., & De Cremer, D. (2004). Autocratic leadership in social
dilemmas: A threat to group stability.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 1-13.
Verweij, K. J., Yang, J., Lahti, J., Veijola, J., Hintsanen, M., Pulkki‐Råback, L., . & Zietsch, B. P. (2012).
Maintenance of genetic variation in human personality: Testing evolutionary models by
estimating heritability due to common causal variants and investigating the effect of distant
inbreeding.
Evolution,
66(10), 3238-3251.
von Rueden, C. (in press). The roots and fruits of social status in small-scale human societies. In J.
Cheng, J. Tracy, & C. Anderson (Eds.)
The Psychology of Social Status. Springer.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 39
von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2011). Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to
dominance and prestige.
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
278(1715),
Willer, R. (2009). Groups reward individual sacrifice: The status solution to the collective action
problem.
American Sociological Review,
74, 23-43.
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: a critique of some current evolutionary
thoughts.
Princeton, New Jersey.
Wilson, D. S., Van Vugt, M., & O'Gorman, R. (2008). Multilevel selection theory and major
evolutionary transitions: Implications for psychological science.
Current Directions in
Psychological Science,
17(1), 6-9.
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: The young male
syndrome.
Ethology and Sociobiology,
6(1), 59-73.
EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF HIERARCHY 40
Figure 1. Chicken: A Game of Status
Note. Payoffs are for Player 1 and 2, respectively; Yield and Dare constitute alternative game strategies (underpinned by genes); game equilibria are indicated with asterisks.
Source: http://twib.in/l/RzER9jLyyyn
LOS ACTOS DE CONFUSIÓN E IMITACIÓN EN EL PROYECTO DE LEY POR EL QUE SE MODIFICA EL RÉGIMEN LEGAL DE LA COMPETENCIA DESLEAL. A. Jorge Viera González Profesor Titular de Derecho Mercantil Universidad Rey Juan Carlos 1. Introducción. El presente trabajo tiene por objetivo exponer de una forma crítica el contenido de la reforma de los actos de confusión e imitación que se contiene en el Proyecto de Ley por
MILPRIT∗: A Constraint-Based Algorithm for Mining Temporal Relational Patterns∗ Sandra de Amo1, Waldecir P. Junior1, Arnaud Giacometti2 1Faculdade de Computac¸˜ao – Universidade Federal de Uberlˆandia (UFU) Campus Santa Mˆonica, Bloco B – Uberlˆandia – MG – Brazil 2Universit´e de Tours, Laboratoire d'Informatique, 2, Place Jean Jaur es 45000 Blois, France,