Owenlab.uwo.ca
This Provisional PDF corresponds to the article as it appeared upon acceptance. Fully formatted
PDF and full text (HTML) versions will be made available soon.
Actigraphy assessments of circadian sleep-wake cycles in the Vegetative and
Minimally Conscious States
BMC Medicine 2013,
11:18
Aurore Thibaut ()
Julia C Nantes ()
Marie-Aurelie Bruno
Olivia Gosseries
Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse ()
Tristan A Bekinschtein
Steven Laureys ()
Article type
Article URL
Like all articles in BMC journals, this peer-reviewed article can be downloaded, printed and
distributed freely for any purposes (see copyright notice below).
Articles in BMC journals are listed in PubMed and archived at PubMed Central.
For information about publishing your research in BMC journals or any BioMed Central journal, go to
2013 Cruse
et al.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Actigraphy assessments of circadian sleep-wake cycles in the Vegetative and
Minimally Conscious States
Damian Cruse1*, Aurore Thibaut2, Athena Demertzi2, Julia C Nantes1, Marie-Aurélie
Bruno2, Olivia Gosseries2, Audrey Vanhaudenhuyse2, Tristan A Bekinschtein3, Adrian M
Owen1 and Steven Laureys2
1Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, 1151 Richmond
Street, London, ON N6A 3K7, Canada.
2Coma Science Group: Department of Neurology and Cyclotron Research Centre,
University Hospital and University of Liège, Allée du 6 août n° 8, Sart Tilman B30 –
3Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Medical Research Council, 15 Chaucer Road,
Cambridge, CB2 7EF, UK.
*Corresponding author
Email addresses:
DC:
[email protected]
AT:
[email protected]
AD:
[email protected]
JCN:
[email protected]
MAB:
[email protected]
OG:
[email protected]
AV:
[email protected]
TAB:
[email protected]
AMO:
[email protected]
SL:
[email protected]
Abstract
Background: The Vegetative and Minimally Conscious States (VS; MCS) are
characterized by absent or highly disordered signs of awareness alongside preserved
sleep-wake cycles. According to international diagnostic guidelines, sleep-wake cycles
are assessed by means of observations of variable periods of eye-opening and eye-
closure. However, there is little empirical evidence for true circadian sleep-wake
cycling in these patients, and there have been no large-scale investigations of the
validity of this diagnostic criterion.
Methods: We measured the circadian sleep-wake rhythms of 55 VS and MCS patients by
means of wrist actigraphy, an indirect method that is highly correlated with
polysomnographic estimates of sleeping/waking.
Results: Contrary to the diagnostic guidelines, a significant proportion of patients did
not exhibit statistically reliable sleep-wake cycles. The circadian rhythms of VS patients
were significantly more impaired than those of MCS patients, as were the circadian
rhythms of patients with non-traumatic injuries relative to those with traumatic
injuries. The reliability of the circadian rhythms were significantly predicted by the
patients' levels of visual and motor functioning, consistent with the putative biological
generators of these rhythms.
Conclusions: The high variability across diagnoses and etiologies highlights the need
for improved guidelines for the assessment of sleep-wake cycles in VS and MCS, and
advocates the use of actigraphy as an inexpensive and non-invasive alternative.
See related commentary here
Keywords: Vegetative State, Minimally Conscious State, circadian rhythms, sleep,
Background
The Vegetative State (VS) or Unresponsive Wakefulness Syndrome (UWS [1]) is thought
to reflect the dissociation of the two primary components of consciousness – awareness
and wakefulness [2, 3]. A common tool for the assessment of awareness is the Coma
Recovery Scale Revised [4] (CRS-R) which includes subscales designed to assess a range
of functions, including auditory, visual, motor, verbal, communication and arousal. A
brain-injured patient is considered to possess awareness if they produce non-reflexive
responses to stimulation, such as tracking an object that is moving in front of the eyes,
or following a verbal command. Patients in the VS do not produce non-reflexive
behavior and are, therefore, considered to lack awareness [5, 6]. Patients in the
Minimally Conscious State (MCS) exhibit some reproducible but inconsistent signs of
awareness, although communication remains absent [6, 7].
Wakefulness, on the other hand, is thought to be preserved in both VS and MCS patients.
According to the standards for VS and MCS outlined by the Multi-Society Task Force for
Permanent Vegetative State [5] and the Royal College of Physicians [6], ‘wakefulness'
refers to the presence of typically cycling periods of eye-closure and eye-opening that
give the appearance of sleep-wake cycles. While a great deal of behavioral and
neuroimaging research has focused on the assumption of unawareness in these patients
[4, 8-10], very little is known regarding the assumption of preserved sleep-wake
A typical sleep-wake cycle follows a circadian rhythm, with a period of between 19- and
28-hours [11]. Electroencephalography (EEG), in combination with other physiological
measures as part of polysomnography, is the gold-standard approach for the
assessment of sleep-wake cycles [12]. However, the results of the limited EEG
investigations of circadian sleep-wake cycling in VS and MCS patients are inconsistent
with the assumption of preserved wakefulness. Landsness
et al. [13] observed sleep-
wake-like changes in the EEG of six MCS patients across one day, while the EEG of five
VS patients remained unchanged between periods of eye-opening and eye-closure.
Isono
et al. [14] also reported an absence of EEG sleep-wake changes in 4 out of 12 VS
patients. High variability has also been observed in other physiological circadian
rhythms in VS and MCS, including body temperature and hormone levels [15, 16], blood
pressure and heart rate [16, 17], and sleep-related erections [18]. Circadian-like
variations in arousal have also been reported in both VS and MCS patients, as indexed
by fluctuating behavioral abilities across the day [19]. Bekinschtein
et al. [20] observed
well-formed circadian rhythms in the body temperatures of two VS patients with
traumatic brain injuries (TBI), but absent rhythms in three VS patients who had
sustained non-traumatic brain injuries (non-TBI), indicating the potential relationship
between etiology and circadian rhythms. It appears, therefore, that, contrary to the
diagnostic guidelines describing these conditions, a great deal of variability exists both
within and across VS and MCS patient groups with regard to the relative preservation of
circadian rhythms.
An indirect and inexpensive approach to detecting circadian sleep-wake cycles from
large numbers of patients is wrist actigraphy, in which a wrist-mounted device is used
to record the frequency and amplitude of motor activity [12]. This method is known to
correlate well with polysomnographic measurements of sleep and wakefulness in
healthy individuals, as well as non-ambulatory patients, such as those with C5 to C7
tetraplegia [21-23]. A number of algorithms have been developed in order to produce
minute-to-minute estimations of sleeping/waking from short-term variations in
actigraphy data in healthy individuals. Broadly, these algorithms judge an individual to
be awake or asleep at a given sample point by weighting the amount of movement in a
number of preceding sample points by a set of predefined constants. Such approaches
have reported between 88 and 97% concordance with polysomnography in healthy
individuals (see [21] for a full review). However, none of these approaches have been
validated with VS or MCS patients by means of concurrent polysomnography and
actigraphy recordings. Nevertheless, a circadian sleep-wake rhythm – that is, more
activity during waking hours and less activity during sleeping hours – can be readily
identified from raw actigraphy recordings, and makes fewer assumptions than these un-
validated algorithms (for example, [24, 25]). In the only article to report actigraphy-
based assessments of sleep-wake rhythms in VS, Bekinschtein
et al. [26] described a
greater deterioration in the circadian rhythmicity evident in the actigraphy of one VS
patient relative to an MCS patient. De Weer
et al. [27] also reported day-night variation
in the amount of movement (as measured by actigraphy) in two TBI MCS patients, but
not in a non-TBI MCS patient. However, in neither of these studies was circadian
rhythmicity examined statistically.
In order to investigate the relative preservation of circadian sleep-wake rhythmicity in
patients in the VS and MCS, we recorded wrist actigraphy from 55 patients (18 VS, 37
MCS) across four days, and subjected the data to cosinor rhythmometry analyses (see
Methods), a standard statistical approach for circadian rhythm identification. By
definition, all of these patients are considered to possess circadian sleep-wake cycles [5-
7]. In keeping with the studies described above, however, we expected to see variability
in the extent to which circadian sleep-wake rhythms were preserved across patients as
a function of etiology (TBI vs. non-TBI) and diagnosis (VS vs. MCS). We also predicted
significant relationships between the behavioral profiles of these patients – as indexed
by their CRS-R subscales – and the relative preservation of their circadian sleep-wake
Patients
Fifty-five patients were recruited from the University Hospital of Liège, Belgium.
Actigraphy recordings were made for at least four days. All patients were VS or MCS.
During their admission, all patients were manually turned in their beds four times per
day. No patient had skin pressure sores that required more frequent manual turning.
No patient required mechanical ventilation. All patients were admitted as part of the
same research protocol, and completed the same tasks across each day, for example,
behavioral tests, positron emission tomography (PET), and magnetic resonance imaging
(MRI). Across their admission, all patients were assessed multiple times with the CRS-R
[4]. The highest CRS-R score and diagnosis across this period are shown in Table 1,
along with other demographic information. In total, 18 VS patients (mean age 38.0, SD
14.8; 7 TBI) and 37 MCS patients (mean age 35.7, SD 15.2; 24 TBI) contributed data to
the study. There was no significant difference in the proportions of each etiology
contributing to the VS and MCS groups. Two two-way ANOVAs with factors of diagnosis
(VS, MCS) and etiology (TBI, non-TBI) conducted on age (in years) and months
post-
ictus revealed only a reliable main effect of etiology on age (F(1, 51) = 10.363,
P <.01)
reflecting the older average age of non-TBI patients. Informed consent was obtained
from the patients' surrogate decision makers. The Ethics Committee of the University
and University Hospital of Liège provided ethical approval for the study.
Procedure
Actigraphy recordings were made with a Philips Actiwatch Spectrum (Philips
Healthcare, Best, Brabant, The Netherlands) attached to the wrist with the highest range
of movement (never the hemiplegic side) for a minimum of four days, sampled in one-
minute epochs. In order to normalize across patients, only the first four days of
actigraphy data were included in the analyses for those patients who were admitted for
longer than four days. The first two hours of data were also excluded to avoid initial
artifacts from attachment of the Actiwatch.
Circadian rhythm analyses
Cosinor rhythmometry analyses [28] were performed on each patient's dataset
individually. This approach uses the least squares method to fit a sine wave with a
period of 24 hours to the raw actigraphy data [11, 12, 28]. The rhythmicity of the fit can
be described by three parameters: the amplitude, the acrophase, and the mesor. The
amplitude of the fit refers to half the distance between the peak and the trough of the
fitted wave – in effect describing the amount of movement produced during periods of
activity. The acrophase describes the point in the cycle at which activity is maximal.
Finally, the mesor (an acronym for
midline-
estimating
statistic
of
rhythm [28])
describes the rhythm-adjusted mean of the wave, or the value around which the fitted
wave oscillates. For equidistant data samples (as employed here), the mesor is
equivalent to the arithmetic mean of the fitted wave, or the average amount of activity
produced across the recording period. The goodness-of-fit of the wave – that is, the
statistical reliability of the circadian rhythm – can also be determined by means of a
zero-amplitude F-test [28].
In order to control for over-fitting of noise to the sine wave, this goodness-of-fit
P-value
was subsequently subjected to a permutation test. Specifically, a set of sine waves with
periods ranging in 10-minute intervals from 6 hours to 48 hours were fit to the data
(excluding rhythms between 19 and 28 hours since these are defined as circadian
periods; see Introduction [11]). The
P-values from these 200 zero-amplitude tests were
then used to form a surrogate distribution to test the hypothesis that a 24-hour rhythm
does not fit the data better than a non-circadian period. When the goodness-of-fit
P-
value associated with the 24-hour rhythm fell below the smallest 5% of surrogate
P-
values, the circadian rhythm was considered to be significant at
P <.05.
A total of 46 out of the whole group of 55 patients (84%) exhibited significant 24-hour
rhythms in their actigraphy data after permutation testing. This proportion is
significantly lower than the diagnostic expectation that all patients retain significant
circadian rhythms (Fisher's Exact Test,
P <.01). When separated according to diagnosis,
15/18 VS patients (83%) and 31/37 MCS patients (84%) returned circadian rhythms
that passed this statistical test. When separated according to etiology, 24/31 TBI
patients (77%) and 22/24 non-TBI patients (92%) exhibited circadian rhythms. There
was no significant effect of diagnosis or etiology on the proportions of patients
exhibiting circadian rhythms (Fisher's Exact Tests, all
P >.14). While age significantly
differed across etiologies, it did not significantly correlate with any of the four
rhythmicity variables (mesor, amplitude, acrophase or goodness-of-fit, as indexed by
the log-transformed zero-amplitude F-ratio).
VS versus MCS patients
Four one-way ANOVAs with diagnosis (VS, MCS) as the factor of interest revealed the
main effects of mesor (F(1,54) = 4.441,
P <.05), amplitude (F(1,54) = 6.819,
P <.05), and
goodness-of-fit (F(1,54) = 16.517,
P <.001), but not acrophase. Together these reflect
the greater average amount of movement across the four days (mesor), the greater
amount of movement during periods of activity (amplitude), and greater statistical
reliability of the circadian rhythms (goodness-of-fit) of MCS patients relative to VS
patients (see Figure 1).
Due to the high inter-correlations between these three significant rhythmicity variables
(all absolute r >.33), all three variables were entered into a backward stepwise logistic
regression in order to determine their relationships with diagnosis, over and above the
effects of the other two variables. This regression retained only goodness-of-fit in the
model as a significant predictor (Wald = 10.189, Beta (SE) = -2.043 (.640),
P <.01),
indicating significantly weaker circadian rhythms in VS patients relative to MCS
patients, regardless of the amount of movement produced by these patients.
Traumatic versus non-traumatic brain injury
Four one-way ANOVAs with etiology (TBI, non-TBI) as the factor of interest revealed
the main effects of amplitude (F(1,54) = 4.299,
P <.05) and goodness-of-fit (F(1,54) =
4.226),
P <.05), but not mesor or acrophase. These effects reflect the greater amount of
movement during periods of activity (amplitude) and the greater statistical reliability of
the circadian rhythms (goodness-of-fit) of TBI patients relative to non-TBI patients.
As with the analyses across diagnosis, due to the high inter-correlations between the
two significant rhythmicity variables, both were entered into a backward stepwise
logistic regression in order to determine their relationships with etiology, over and
above the effect of the other variable. This regression retained neither variable as a
significant predictor, likely due to the weak effects of etiology on these variables
(contrast F-values above with those in the analyses across diagnosis).
Relationship between rhythmicity and behavioral profile
Four backward linear regressions were conducted on the four rhythmicity variables
with the six subscales of the CRS-R as predictors. Diagnosis was also included as a
predictor since the higher scores on each subscale are also more likely to be associated
with MCS and the lower scores with VS. The motor sub-scale was found to significantly
predict mesor (F(1,54) = 7.792,
P <.01, B(SE) = 6.174 (2.212),
P <.01) and amplitude
(F(2,54) = 6.178,
P <.01, B(SE) = 3.462 (1.453),
P <.05). The visual sub-scale was found
to significantly predict acrophase (F(1,54) = 4.636,
P <.05, B(SE) = -.108 (.050),
P <.05),
and both the visual and motor subscale together were found to predict goodness-of-fit
(F(2,54) = 16.487,
P <.001, B-visual(SE) = .208 (.055),
P <.001, B-motor(SE) = .225
(.071),
P <.005).
Discussion
On the basis of periodic eye-opening and eye-closure, patients in the VS and MCS are
considered to have preserved circadian sleep-wake rhythms [5, 6]. However, by means
of an indirect measure of sleep-wake rhythmicity – wrist actigraphy – we have shown
that a significant proportion of these patients do not exhibit statistically reliable
circadian sleep-wake rhythms. The observed variability across patients is consistent
with previous smaller studies of circadian rhythmicity in VS and MCS (see Background),
and is the first evidence from a large-scale study of sleep-wake cycling using the
inexpensive and non-invasive method of wrist actigraphy.
While there was no significant difference in the proportion of patients exhibiting
significant sleep-wake rhythms between VS and MCS patients, the goodness-of-fit of the
circadian rhythms in the data of MCS patients were significantly higher than those of the
VS patients (see Figure 1). This result indicates that the circadian sleep-wake cycles of
MCS patients were significantly more statistically reliable than those of VS patients.
Importantly, this remained true when taking into account the morphology of the rhythm
(that is, its mesor and amplitude), indicating that the effect of diagnosis on the statistical
reliability of the circadian rhythms is not driven by simple differences in the amount
that a patient moves, but rather reflects differences in the circadian rhythmicity with
which this movement occurs.
A master biological clock in the hypothalamic suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) is
considered to maintain the timing of circadian rhythms. The SCN in turn modulates the
activity of the ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) – a circuit of subcortical
nuclei responsible for promoting wakefulness (see [29] for a review). One region of the
ARAS – the central thalamus – is known to be crucial for the regulation of arousal and
has been linked to the disorders of consciousness exhibited by VS and MCS patients
[30]. Indeed, the extent of atrophy in this region of the thalamus has been associated
with the degree of disability exhibited by these patients [31]. More broadly, greater
thalamic atrophy has been observed in VS patients relative to MCS patients using
in vivo
diffusion tensor imaging [32] (DTI). The weaker circadian sleep-wake rhythms
observed in the VS patients in the current study are, therefore, entirely consistent with
these differential patterns of damage to the thalamus.
Etiology was also shown to have a small effect on the amount that patients moved
during periods of activity (amplitude) and the statistical reliability of the circadian
rhythm (goodness-of-fit). Similarly, Bekinschtein
et al. [20] observed reliable circadian
temperature rhythms in only TBI VS patients, but not in non-TBI patients, while De
Weer
et al. [27] detected sleep-wake activity changes in only TBI MCS patients. The
primary neuropathology associated with TBI is diffuse axonal injury with relative
preservation of the cortex, while non-TBI involves more widespread damage to the
cortex and basal ganglia [33-39]. The greater impairment of circadian rhythms in non-
TBI patients relative to TBI patients reported here is, therefore, consistent with the
general patterns of neuropathology associated with the two etiologies. Indeed, mouse
models of hypoxic brain injury have been shown to result in impaired sleep-wake
Significant relationships were also observed between the behavioral profiles of the
patients – as indexed by their CRS-R sub-scales – and aspects of their circadian
rhythmicities. A significant positive relationship was found between the motor subscale
and the mesor and amplitude of the rhythm. The motor subscale of the CRS-R is scored
from flaccid motor tone at its lowest, to object manipulation and automatic motor
responses at its highest (before emergence from MCS). Since wrist movements were
used to indirectly measure the circadian rhythms, it is unsurprising that greater
amounts of movement exhibited by patients across the recording period (mesor,
amplitude) are related to their overall abilities to produce motor output during
behavioral assessments. This result suggests the need for caution in the use of
actigraphy for assessing circadian sleep-wake rhythms since they rely on motor output
for a rhythm to be detected. Nevertheless, our analyses have demonstrated that
significant changes in the statistical reliability of the rhythms across diagnoses are not
dependent on the amount of movement produced, suggesting that actigraphy can be
used to assess the statistical reliability of circadian sleep-wake cycles, regardless of the
degree of activity exhibited by the patients.
A combination of the visual and motor subscales significantly predicted the goodness-
of-fit of the circadian rhythms. The visual subscale score describes behaviors from
absent visual startle at its lowest, through fixation and pursuit, to object recognition at
its highest. This relationship is of particular interest since the master clock for circadian
rhythms, the SCN, is itself timed by light inputs from the retina during the day, as well as
melatonin from the pineal gland at night [29]. The more purposeful eye-movements of
those scoring high on the visual subscale may allow for differing levels of light to reach
the retina – perhaps through a greater ability to orient toward light or to maintain eye-
opening for longer periods – and, consequently, result in a strengthening of the rhythm
via the SCN. The predictive value of the visual subscale could, therefore, be considered
to be consistent with our understanding of the biological generators of sleep-wake
rhythmicity. This conclusion is necessarily speculative, however, since it is unclear
whether high visual functioning is associated with a greater degree of orientation
toward light or longer periods of eye-opening. Further investigation of this relationship
will contribute to our understanding of the exogenous cues that drive circadian rhythms
in VS/MCS patients.
A significant relationship was also found between the visual subscale of the CRS-R and
the acrophase (time of maximal activity) of the rhythm, over and above the contribution
of the other CRS-R subscales, or of the diagnosis of the patient. The relationship with
acrophase reflects the tendency for patients with higher visual functioning to be most
active later in the afternoon than patients with lower visual functioning (Visual Score >=
1, Mean acrophase (SD) 18:20 (three hours); Visual Score = 0, Mean acrophase (SD)
17:20 (two hours)). Consistent with this observation, exposure to higher levels of light
has been associated with later peaks of activity in institutionalized individuals [41, 42].
However, the activity peaks of healthy individuals occur earlier in the day than those
observed in the patients here, typically between approximately 13:30p.m. and
approximately 16:00p.m. [24]. It has been observed that the levels of light experienced
by institutionalized patients are considerably lower than those of non-institutionalized
individuals [42, 43], and since the patients in the current study were residing on a
hospital ward during the recording period, it is likely they were exposed to abnormally
fluctuating levels of light compared with healthy individuals. Unfortunately, we were
unable to record light levels alongside actigraphy; however, future studies investigating
their contribution to the timing of activity of VS and MCS patients will be invaluable.
Since we inferred the circadian rhythms of patients from wrist actigraphy, it is likely
that the recordings contain some levels of exogenous activity, perhaps from nurses
moving the patient from bed to chair. Since these patients were all admitted to the
same ward of the University Hospital of Liège as part of the same research protocol,
they all received equivalent levels of care and were involved in the same assessments
throughout the day – for example, behavioral tests, PET and MRI. As a result, the
potential exogenous noise in the data would then be equally distributed across all
patients. Our conclusions regarding the effects of diagnosis, etiology and behavioral
profile on sleep-wake cycles, therefore, would remain valid despite this potential
confound. The use of simultaneous video-recordings would allow for the exclusion of
activity that is generated exogenously and would further validate our findings.
Some prescribed medications may also have an effect on actigraphy-detected circadian
rhythms. For example, treatment for spasticity (for example, with baclofen) is common
in VS/MCS patients and may increase the amount of movement that will be detected
with actigraphy, while psychoactive medications (for example, amantadine) may also
serve to exogenously modulate a patient's level of arousal. Caution in this regard is not
limited to actigraphy, however, since psychoactive medications will also alter the
resting EEG of a patient, thereby modulating the level of wakefulness that will be
inferred from polysomnography. Due to differences in the wishes of families and
physicians, a wide variety of medications are prescribed to VS and MCS patients (see
Table 2 for details). As a result, it is not possible to statistically control for each of these
drugs individually, nor for their many interactions. Nevertheless, there is no reason to
believe that prescribed medications would systematically differ between VS and MCS
groups due to the paucity of treatment recommendations for all patients with disorders
of consciousness ([44]). Future controlled clinical trials are needed in order to provide
insights into the effects of specific medications not only on circadian rhythmicity, but
also on VS/MCS patient outcome in general.
A final caveat is that the apparent absence of reliable circadian rhythms in some of our
patients may be a result of a lack of sensitivity of the actigraphy method, rather than the
true absence of those rhythms. While wrist actigraphy has been validated for sleep
assessment in patients with C5 to C7 tetraplegia [23], these patients are nevertheless
capable of small but purposeful wrist movements. Patients in the VS, however, are by
definition unable to produce purposeful movements, although spontaneous movements
are common. Similarly, due to the heterogeneity of brain injuries of these patients, it is
not clear whether the presence of actigraphy-detected waking is necessarily indicative
of concurrent cerebral waking. Future validation of the relationship between
polysomnography and actigraphy measures of sleeping and waking in VS and MCS
patients is needed in order to fully characterize the nature of their circadian rhythms.
Conclusions
Our analyses indicate a greater impairment of circadian sleep-wake cycling in patients
in the VS compared with those in the MCS, and in those with non-TBI compared with
TBI. The significant differences observed between VS and MCS patients support the
conclusion that these are diagnostically distinct entities. However, they also suggest
that despite periods of eye-closure and eye-opening, sleep-wake cycles are not
necessarily present despite the clinical criteria for these conditions [5-7]. Wrist
actigraphy is considerably less expensive and less invasive than other forms of sleep-
wake monitoring and may, therefore, provide a reliable means of determining the
extent to which these cycles are preserved in individual patients. These recordings
could also allow clinicians and researchers to identify the time of day in which a patient
is most active, in order to schedule behavioral and/or neuroimaging assessments for a
time that maximizes the likelihood of detecting an appropriate response (see [26]).
Future validation of the relationship between actigraphy and polysomnography
measures of sleeping/waking in VS and MCS patients will allow for a more complete
understanding of the physiological nature of these circadian rhythms. Follow-up
studies will also determine the prognostic utility of wrist actigraphy for VS and MCS
ARAS, ascending reticular activating system; CRS-R, Coma Recovery Scale Revised; DTI,
diffusion tensor imaging; EEG, electroencephalography; MCS, Minimally Conscious
State; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; non-TBI, non-traumatic brain injuries; PET,
positron emission tomography; SCN, suprachiasmatic nuclei; TBI, traumatic brain
injuries; UWS, Unresponsive Wakefulness Syndrome; VS, Vegetative State
Competing interests
All authors declare no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
DC designed and conducted the analyses and wrote the manuscript. AT, AD, MAB, OG
and AV collected all data and contributed to the final manuscript. JCN contributed to the
analyses and the final manuscript. TAB, AMO and SL provided conceptual input and
contributed to the final manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the
This research was supported by generous funding from the European Commission,
Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique, the Mind Science Foundation, the Belgian French-
Speaking Community Concerted Research Action, University Hospital of Liège,
University of Liège, the Wellcome Trust, the Canada Excellence Research Chair
Programme (CERC), and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR).
References
Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F, Lavrijsen J, Leon-Carrion J, Sannita WG, Sazbon
L, Schmutzhard E, von Wild KR, Zeman A, Dolce G:
Unresponsive wakefulness
syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apallic syndrome.
BMC
Med 2010,
8:68.
Laureys S:
The neural correlate of (un)awareness: lessons from the
vegetative state.
Trends Cogn Sci 2005,
9:556-559.
Jennett B, Plum F:
Persistent vegetative state after brain damage. A
syndrome in search of a name.
Lancet 1972,
1:734-737.
Giacino JT, Kalmar K, Whyte J:
The JFK Coma Recovery Scale-Revised:
measurement characteristics and diagnostic utility.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil
2004,
85:2020-2029.
The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS:
Medical aspects of the persistent
vegetative state (1). N Engl J Med 1994,
330:1499-1508.
Royal College of Physicians:
The permanent vegetative state. Review by a
working group convened by the Royal College of Physicians and endorsed
by the Conference of Medical Royal Colleges and their faculties of the
United Kingdom.
J R Coll Physicians Lond 1996 (Updated 2003),
30:119-121.
Giacino JT, Ashwal S, Childs N, Cranford R, Jennett B, Katz DI, Kelly JP, Rosenberg
JH, Whyte J, Zafonte RD, Zasler ND:
The minimally conscious state: definition
and diagnostic criteria.
Neurology 2002,
58:349-353.
Owen AM, Coleman MR, Boly M, Davis MH, Laureys S, Pickard JD:
Detecting
awareness in the vegetative state.
Science 2006,
313:1402.
Monti MM, Vanhaudenhuyse A, Coleman MR, Boly M, Pickard JD, Tshibanda L,
Owen AM, Laureys S:
Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of
consciousness.
N Engl J Med 2010,
362:579-589.
Cruse D, Chennu S, Chatelle C, Bekinschtein TA, Fernández-Espejo D, Pickard JD,
Laureys S, Owen AM:
Bedside detection of awareness in the vegetative state -
a cohort study.
Lancet 2011,
378:2088-2094.
Refinetti R:
Circadian Physiology. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis Group; 2006.
Ancoli-Israel S, Cole R, Alessi C, Chambers M, Moorcroft W, Pollak CP:
The role of
actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms.
Sleep 2003,
26:342-
Landsness E, Bruno MA, Noirhomme Q, Riedner B, Gosseries O, Schnakers C,
Massimini M, Laureys S, Tononi G, Boly M:
Electrophysiological correlates of
behavioural changes in vigilance in vegetative state and minimally
conscious state.
Brain 2011,
134:2222-2232.
Isono M, Wakabayashi Y, Fujiki MM, Kamida T, Kobayashi H:
Sleep cycle in
patients in a state of permanent unconsciousness.
Brain Inj 2002,
16:705-
Fukudome Y, Abe I, Saku Y, Matsumura K, Sadoshima S, Utunomiya H, Fujishima
M:
Circadian blood pressure in patients in a persistent vegetative state.
Am J
Physiol 1996,
270:R1109-1114.
Pattoneri P, Tirabassi G, Pela G, Astorri E, Mazzucchi A, Borghetti A:
Circadian
blood pressure and heart rate changes in patients in a persistent vegetative
state after traumatic brain injury.
J Clin Hypertens (Greenwich) 2005,
7:734-
Guan J, You C, Liu Y, Zhang R, Wang Z:
Characteristics of infradian and
circadian rhythms in the persistent vegetative state.
J Int Med Res 2011,
Oksenberg A, Arons E, Sazbon L, Mizrahi A, Radwan H:
Sleep-related erections
in vegetative state patients.
Sleep 2000,
23:953-957.
Candelieri A, Cortese MD, Dolce G, Riganello F, Sannita WG:
Visual pursuit:
within-day variability in the severe disorder of consciousness.
J
Neurotrauma 2011,
28:2013-2017.
Bekinschtein TA, Golombek DA, Simonetta SH, Coleman MR, Manes FF:
Circadian rhythms in the vegetative state.
Brain Inj 2009,
23:915-919.
de Souza L, Benedito-Silva AA, Pires ML, Poyares D, Tufik S, Calil HM:
Further
validation of actigraphy for sleep studies.
Sleep 2003,
26:81-85.
Berger AM, Wielgus KK, Young-McCaughan S, Fischer P, Farr L, Lee KA:
Methodological challenges when using actigraphy in research.
J Pain
Symptom Manage 2008,
36:191-199.
Spivak E, Oksenberg A, Catz A:
The feasibility of sleep assessment by
actigraph in patients with tetraplegia.
Spinal Cord 2007,
45:765-770.
Brown A, Smolensky M, D'Alonzo G, Redmond D, Conrad E, Hsi B:
Circadian
rhythm in human activity objectively quantified by actigraphy.
Prog Clin Biol
Res 1990,
341A:77-83.
Girardin J-L, Kripke DF, Ancoli-Israel S, Klauber MR, Sepulveda RS, Mowen M-A,
Assmus JD, Langer RD:
Circadian sleep, illumination, and activity patterns in
women: influences of aging and time reference.
Physiol Behav 2000,
68:347-
Bekinschtein T, Cologan V, Dahmen B, Golombek D:
You are only coming
through in waves: wakefulness variability and assessment in patients with
impaired consciousness.
Prog Brain Res 2009,
177:171-189.
De Weer AS, Da Ros M, Berré J, Mélot C, Goldman S, Peigneux P:
Environmental
influences on activity patterns in altered states of consciousness.
Eur J
Neurol 2011,
18:1432-1434.
Nelson W, Tong YL, Lee JK, Halberg F:
Methods for cosinor-rhythmometry.
Chronobiologia 1979,
6:305-323.
Saper CB, Scammell TE, Lu J:
Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and circadian
rhythms.
Nature 2005,
437:1257-1263.
Schiff ND:
Central thalamic contributions to arousal regulation and
neurological disorders of consciousness.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008,
1129:105-
Fernandez-Espejo D, Junque C, Bernabeu M, Roig-Rovira T, Vendrell P, Mercader
JM:
Reductions of thalamic volume and regional shape changes in the
vegetative and the minimally conscious states.
J Neurotrauma 2010,
27:1187-
Fernández-Espejo D, Bekinschtein T, Monti MM, Pickard JD, Junque C, Coleman
MR, Owen AM:
Diffusion weighted imaging distinguishes the vegetative state
from the minimally conscious state.
NeuroImage 2011,
54:103-112.
Adams JH, Graham DI, Murray LS, Scott G:
Diffuse axonal injury due to
nonmissile head-injury in humans - an analysis of 45 cases.
Ann Neurol 1982,
12:557-563.
Kinney HC, Samuels MA:
Neuropathology of the persistent vegetative state. A
review.
J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1994,
53:548-558.
Adams JH, Jennett B, McLellan DR, Murray LS, Graham DI:
The neuropathology
of the vegetative state after head injury.
J Clin Pathol 1999,
52:804-806.
Jennett B, Adams JH, Murray LS, Graham DI:
Neuropathology in vegetative and
severely disabled patients after head injury.
Neurology 2001,
56:486-490.
Adams JH, Duchen LW:
Greenfield's Neuropathology, 5th edition. New York, New
York: Oxford University Press; 1992.
Adams JH, Graham DI, Jennett B:
The neuropathology of the vegetative state
after an acute brain insult.
Brain 2000,
123:1327-1338.
Kinney HC, Korein J, Panigrahy A, Dikkes P, Goode R:
Neuropathological
findings in the brain of Karen Ann Quinlan. The role of the thalamus in the
persistent vegetative state.
N Engl J Med 1994,
330:1469-1475.
Veasey SC, Davis CW, Fenik P, Zhan G, Hsu YJ, Pratico D, Gow A:
Long-term
intermittent hypoxia in mice: protracted hypersomnolence with oxidative
injury to sleep-wake brain regions.
Sleep 2004,
27:194-201.
Shochat T, Martin J, Marler M, Ancoli-Israel S:
Illumination levels in nursing
home patients: effects on sleep and activity rhythms.
J Sleep Res 2000,
9:373-
Ancoli-Israel S, Martin JL, Kripke DF, Marler M, Klauber MR:
Effect of light
treatment on sleep and circadian rhythms in demented nursing home
patients.
J Am Geriatr Soc 2002,
50:282-289.
Campbell SS, Kripke DF, Gillin JC, Hrubovcak JC:
Exposure to light in healthy
elderly subjects and Alzheimer's patients.
Physiol Behav 1988,
42:141-144.
Giacino J, Whyte J:
The vegetative and minimally conscious states: current
knowledge and remaining questions.
J Head Trauma Rehabil 2005,
20:30-50.
Figure 1. Actigraphy data from four representative patients. Each panel shows
intensity of activity across each recording day. Red lines indicate the fit of the circadian
rhythm. Note the periodic structure of the activity of the two patients with significant
rhythms (left), compared with those without (right). Patients 19, 21, 52 and 45 are
shown (clockwise from top-left). Log activity data smoothed across five minutes is
plotted for clarity of visualization.
Table 1. Demographics and circadian rhythm fits for all patients.
Patient Gender
Etiology
Mesor Acrophase Amplitude Sig.
ID
Fit?
The final column indicates whether the circadian rhythm fit was significant or not. MCS,
Minimally Conscious State; TBI, Traumatic Brain Injury; VS, Vegetative State
Table 2. Patient etiology and prescribed daily medications.
Specific etiology
Daily medication dosage
ID
1 x Clonazepam 2 mg 1 x Phenytoin 100 mg 1 x Clopidogrel 75 mg 1 x Acetylcysteine 600 mg 3 x Baclofen 25 mg 3 x Levetiracetam 500 mg
1 x Amantadine 100 mg 2 x Clonidine 150 mg 1 x Bisopropol 2.5 mg 1 x Paroxetine 20 mg 1 x Tetrazepam 50 mg 1 x Acetylcysteine 600 mg
2 x Lamotrigine 50 mg 1 x Levetiracetam 1,000 mg 3 x Diazepam 5 mg 3 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 1 x Domperidome 10 mg
1 x Vancomycin 2,000 mg 4 x Piperacillin 4,000 mg 4 x Amikacin 1,000 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 50 mg 3 x Paracetamol 1,000 mg 1 x Diazepam 10 mg 3 x Baclofen 15 mg 1 x Acetylcysteine 600 mg
3 x Valproic Acid 40 ml 3 x Diazepam 10 mg 2 x Terbutaline 5 mg 3 x Baclofen 10 mg 3 x Dantrolene 25 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg 2 x Fluconazole 50 mg 1 x Aspirin 160 mg
1 x Amantadine 100 mg 1 x Zolpidem 10 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 1 x Trihexyphenidyl 4 mg
Tumor / Hemorrhage
2 x Lamotrigine 100 mg 1 x Aspirin 80 mg 1 x Omeprazole 20 mg 1 x Escitalopram 10 mg
1 x Aspirin 100 mg 3 x Tizanidine 4 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg 1 x Omeprazole 20 mg
1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 2 x Valproic Acid 500 mg
1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 3 x Valproic Acid 2 ml 2 x Levetiracetam 7.5 ml 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
3 x Baclofen 10 mg 1 x Omeprazole 10 mg 4 x Domperidone 1 mg
2 x Clonazepam 1 mg
1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
2 x Carbamazepine 200 mg
1 x Valproic Acid 500 mg 1 x Lansoprazole 20 mg
1 x Phenobarbital 100 mg 3 x Baclofen 10 mg
1 x Valproic Acid 500 mg 1 x Bisoprolol 5 mg 3 x Piracetam 1,200 mg 3 x Baclofen 25 mg
1 x Omeprazole 20 mg 2 x Flecainide 100 mg 2 x Levetiracetam 10 ml 3 x Baclofen 10 mg 2 x Sodium Valproate 600 ml 1 x Clonazepam 2 mg
1 x Escitalopram 10 mg 4 x Alprazolam 250 mg 1 x Trazodone 100 mg 1 x Prothipendyl 80 mg
2 x Baclofen 10 mg 1 x Paracetamol 500 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 40 mg 2 x Levetiracetam 7.5 ml
1 x Bisopropol 2.5 mg 1 x Pantoprazole 20 mg 2 x Tizanidine 2 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
2 x Ranitidine 150 mg 3 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
3 x Baclofen 25 mg 2 x Tizanidine 4 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 20 mg 1 x Amantadine 50 mg
3 x Baclofen 25 mg 3 x Domperidome 10 mg 2 x Clonazepam 2.5 mg 1 x Promethazine 16 mg
1 x Amantadine 100 mg 2 x Baclofen 10 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 1 x Tizanidine 4 mg
2 x Valproic Acid 7.5 ml 1 x Lamotrigine 25 mg 3 x Baclofen 10 mg 2 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 20 mg
1 x Esomeprazole 40 mg 3 x Clonazepam 2 mg 3 x Paracetamol 1,000 mg 2 x Levetiracetam 500 mg 3 x Benserazide 250 mg
1 x Acetylcysteine 600 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 1 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Atenolol 50 mg
1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg 1 x Glycopyrrolate 10 mg
1 x Amantadine 100 mg 1 x Paroxetine 20 mg 3 x Domperidone 10 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg
2 x Bisopropol 2.5 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 1 x Amantadine 100 mg 3 x Metamizole 500 mg 3 x Meropenem 1,000 mg 3 x Ciprofloxacin 400 mg
3 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Amantadine 100 mg 1 x Escitalopram 10 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
1 x Omeprazole 20 mg 1 x Ranitidine 300 mg 3 x Valproic Acid 2 ml 1 x Sertraline 2.5 mg 2 x Baclofen 5 ml
2 x Carbamazepine 400 mg
1 x Lansoprazole 30 mg 1 x Topiramate 150 mg 2 x Modafinil 100 mg 1 x Aniracetam 1,500 mg
3 x Levetiracetam 500 mg 3 x Baclofen 10 mg 1 x Atenolol 50 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 3 x Indometacin 50 mg
2 x Ranitidine 150 mg 1 x Acetylcysteine 200 mg 3 x Baclofen 10 mg 1 x Escitalopram 10 mg 1 x Amantadine 100 mg 2 x Diclofenac 50 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
1 x Acetylcysteine 600 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 1 x Baclofen 10 mg
1 x Baclofen 10 mg 1 x Trazodone 25 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
1 x Levetiracetam 3,000 mg 1 x Phenobarbital 100 mg 1 x Esomeprazole 40 mg 1 x Simvastatin 40 mg 1 x Aspirin 100 mg 1 x Escitalopram 10 mg
1 x Levothyroxine 25 mg 1 x Carbamazepine 200 mg
1 x Ranitidine 300 mg 3 x Phenobarbital 100 mg 3 x Baclofen 25 mg 5 x Lorazepam 2.5 mg 1 x Phenytoin 100 mg
1 x Amiodarone 200 mg
1 x Clopidogrel 75 mg 3 x Baclofen 25 mg 2 x Diltiazem 60 mg 1 x Trazodone 100 mg 1 x Lormetazepam 2 mg 2 x Prazepam 10 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg 2 x Acetylcysteine 400 mg 3 x Dantrolene 25 mg
1 x Bisopropol 5 mg 3 x Baclofen 10 mg 1 x Levothyroxine 50 mg 1 x Prednisolone 5 mg
Meningitis
1 x Moxifloxacin 500 mg 1 x Levetiracetam 500 mg 2 x Ranitidine 150 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
2 x Levetiracetam 1,000 mg 1 x Phenytoin 500 mg 6 x Valproic Acid 6.5 ml 1 x Lorazepam 1 mg 1 x Ranitidine 300 mg 2 x Enoxaparin Sodium 60 mg
3 x Valproic Acid 600 mg 1 x Ranitidine 150 mg
Cardio-respiratory
2 x Acetylcysteine 200 mg
1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 60 mg 1 x Ranitidine 10 ml
1 x Atenolol 25 mg 2 x Modafinil 100 mg
3 x Baclofen 10 mg 1 x Diazepam 5 mg 1 x Prazepam 5 mg 2 x Omeprazole 20 mg 1 x Levocetirizine 10 mg
3 x Dantrolene 100 mg 3 x Carbamazepine 200 mg 3 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Omeprazole 20 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
3 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Pantoprazole 20 mg 3 x Dantrolene 100 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
3 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Trazodone 100 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg 2 x Levetiracetam 5 ml
4 x Paracetamol 500 mg 3 x Baclofen 10 mg 2 x Esomeprazole 20 mg 4 x Acetylcysteine 300 mg
2 x Oxcarbazepine 450 mg 1 x Levetiracetam 5 ml 1 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Tizanidine 4 mg
2 x Levetiracetam 100 mg
2 x Ranitidine 150 mg 2 x Lamotrigine 25 mg 3 x Baclofen 25 mg 1 x Enoxaparin Sodium 40 mg
Source: http://www.owenlab.uwo.ca/pdf/2013-Cruse-BioMed%20Central-Actigraphy%20assessments%20of%20circadian%20sleep-wake%20cycles%20in%20the%20Vegetative%20and%20Minimally%20Conscious%20States.pdf
Get inside to spot the BIG moves in SMALL COMPANIES SMALL "This year was our fiscal cliff. FY14 is when we realise all the hard work from our stategy. " Richard Pang, CEO ISS Group Welcome back to another year of Under the Radar. SHARE RESEARCH Tip 1. p2 mAynE pHARmA (myx)
Hahr and Molitch Clinical Diabetes and Endocrinology (2015) 1:2 DOI 10.1186/s40842-015-0001-9 Management of diabetes mellitus in patients withchronic kidney disease Allison J. Hahr and Mark E. Molitch* Glycemic control is essential to delay or prevent the onset of diabetic kidney disease. There are a number ofglucose-lowering medications available but only a fraction of them can be used safely in chronic kidney diseaseand many of them need an adjustment in dosing. The ideal target hemoglobin A1c is approximately 7 % but thistarget is adjusted based on the needs of the patient. Diabetes control should be optimized for each individualpatient, with measures to reduce diabetes-related complications and minimize adverse events. Overall care ofdiabetes necessitates attention to multiple aspects, including reducing the risk of cardiovascular disease, and often,multidisciplinary care is needed.