Microsoft word - send.doc
Second to Fourth Digit Length Ratio (2D:4D) and Adult Sex Hormone Levels: New Data and a
Meta-Analytic Review
Running title: 2D:4D and adult sex hormone levels
[
Accepted for Publication in Psychoneuroendocrinology. Final Unedited Draft ]
Johannes Hönekopp*, Luise Bartholdt
Technische Universität Chemnitz, Institut für Psychologie, Wilhelm-Raabe Str. 43, D-09120
Lothar Beier, Andreas Liebert
Klinikum Chemnitz, Institut für Laboratoriumsmedizin, Flemmingstr. 2, D-09009 Chemnitz
* Corresponding author. Address: Technische Universität Chemnitz, Institut für Psychologie,
Wilhelm-Raabe Str. 43, D-09120 Chemnitz, Germany; e-mail:
[email protected]
chemnitz.de; phone: ++49 (371) 531 63 99; fax: ++49 (371) 531 63 33.
The relative length of the second (index) to the fourth (ring) finger (2D:4D) is a putative negative
correlate of prenatal testosterone (T) exposure. Therefore, 2D:4D (and to a lesser extent Dr-l, the
difference between 2D:4D in the right hand and in the left hand) has often been used to study
effects of prenatal androgenization on human behavior and cognition. However, evidence
suggests that 2D:4D may also be related to levels of circulating sex hormones in adults. This
would question the validity of 2D:4D as a means of studying the effects of prenatal sex
hormones. Here we present new data from two non-clinical samples (64 women and 102 men)
regarding the relationships of 2D:4D and Dr-l with circulating sex hormone levels. We then
present a meta-analytic review of all the present evidence regarding this issue. The results suggest
that, in the normal population, 2D:4D and Dr-l are not associated with adult sex hormone levels.
The findings from this current study add to the growing body of evidence demonstrating that
2D:4D is a suitable tool to study the effects of prenatal androgenization on human behavior and
Keywords: 2D:4D; finger length; prenatal androgenization; sex hormones; testosterone
The ratio of the length of the second (index) to the forth (ring) finger, 2D:4D, is sexually
dimorphic in humans. Averaged across samples from various populations, female values were
found to be about .25 standard deviations higher than male values (Manning et al., 2000).
Several lines of evidence suggest that foetal sex hormone (especially androgen) levels
bring about the sex difference in 2D:4D and also affect intra-sexual variability in this measure.
First, the sex difference in 2D:4D is already observable at the end of the first trimester of foetal
development (Malas et al., 2006). Second, the development of both, genitals and digits, is
controlled by the same genes HoxA and HoxD (Kondo et al., 1997). Third, the sex difference in
2D:4D appears unaffected by puberty as is evidenced by cross sectional (Manning et al., 1998)
and longitudinal (McIntyre et al., 2005; Trivers et al., 2006) data. Fourth, right hand 2D:4D at the
age of two years was found to be negatively correlated with the T/estrogen ratio as measured by
amniocentesis in the second trimester (Lutchmaya et al, 2004). Fifth, individual 2D:4D values
were shown to have high longitudinal stability from age 10 to age 14 (Trivers et al., 2006) and
some stability from infancy throughout to age 17 (McIntyre et al., 2005). Sixth, children with
congenital adrenal hyperplasia, a condition that results in abnormally high T levels during
gestation, were found to have lower 2D:4D values than normal controls in two studies (Brown et
al., 2002; Ökten et al., 2002). A third study did not find such a difference (Buck et al., 2003), but
this study shows some methodological shortcomings (for a thorough critique, see McIntyre,
2006). Seventh, females with male co-twins appear to be exposed to higher T levels in utero and
they have lower 2D:4D values than females with female co-twins (van Anders et al., 2006).
Eighth, 2D:4D negatively correlates with T sensitivity in androgen receptors as measured by the
number of CAG repeats in the androgen receptor gene (Manning et al., 2003). Ninth, autism is
believed to result from prenatal hypermasculinization of the brain, and autistic subjects were
found to have lower 2D:4D than normal controls (Manning et al., 2001).
For these reasons, 2D:4D may be a valid marker of prenatal T exposure. As prenatal T
affects human behavior and cognition and since other ways of studying these effects in humans
are laborious and pose various difficulties (Cohen-Bendahan et al., 2005; Collaer and Hines,
1995) 2D:4D has become popular as a means to study the effects of prenatal androgenization in
humans, especially regarding sex-linked behaviors and traits (Manning, 2002a). In some areas,
findings regarding the relationship between 2D:4D and research variables appear quite diverse.
For example, a negative relationship between mental rotation ability and 2D:4D has been found
for females but not for males (Burton et al., 2005), for males but nor for females (Sanders et al.,
2005), or for neither sex (Alexander, 2006; Coolican and Peters, 2003; Falter et al., 2006). In other
areas, research results have been remarkably consistent. For example, al studies focusing at
2D:4D and athletic ability have found negative relationships (Hönekopp et al., 2006b; Manning,
2002a, 2002b; Pokrywka et al., 2005; Paul et al., 2006). Interestingly, evidence for homologous
effects of prenatal steroids as indicated by 2D:4D and of circulating steroids has been found in
some fields. For example, not only prenatal T but also circulating T appears to boost sport
performance in men (Neave and Wolfson, 2003); and both, low prenatal androgenization as
indicated by high 2D:4D values and high levels of circulating estrogen, appear to increase the risk
of eating disorders in women (Klump et al., 2006).
In sum, 2D:4D is promising as a marker for studying effects of prenatal androgenization
in humans (for a more thourough discussion, see McIntyre, 2006). However, questions remain
open. For example, it is unclear why the sex difference in 2D:4D is so low (about .25 standard
deviations) whereas the sex difference in prenatal T levels appears to be much higher (
d ≈1.9; cf.
Knickmeyer et al., 2005; van de Beek et al., 2004). This difference in effect sizes suggests that
other, yet to be discovered, factors than prenatal T affect 2D:4D and that 2D:4D may not be a
very accurate indicator of prenatal T. To clarify the latter point, more longitudinal studies
comparing prenatal T with 2D:4D values later in life would be of great value.
Here, we focus on another possible problem of 2D:4D: Potential associations between
2D:4D and levels of circulating sex hormones threaten its validity as a measure of prenatal
androgenization as we argue below. Our aim is therefore to clarify the relationship between
2D:4D and circulating sex hormone levels. In order to do so, we present new data regarding
associations between 2D:4D and current levels of various sex hormones in women and men. We
then review the literature on the relationship between sex hormone levels and 2D:4D, using
meta-analytic procedures when appropriate. However, before we turn to the new data and the
literature review, we show why potential associations between adult sex hormone levels and
2D:4D would threaten the validity of the latter as a means of studying prenatal androgenization,
and we argue why such associations might be expected.
As 2D:4D appears to reflect prenatal T levels, a correlation between 2D:4D and X (the
study variable of interest) is usually interpreted as indicating an effect of prenatal T on X (see
Figure 1a). Although this interpretation is plausible, it is not necessarily correct. 2D:4D may not
exclusively reflect prenatal androgenization but also adult levels of sex hormones (see below). In
this case, an observed relationship between 2D:4D and X may not arise from prenatal T but from
activational effects of circulating sex hormones on X, from effects of X on circulating sex
hormones, or from bidirectional causation (Figure 1b). Of course, an association between
circulating adult hormones and 2D:4D does not undermine possible prenatal hormonal
influences on digit ratios. But in this case, it may be wrong to conclude, on grounds of an
observed relationship between the study variable and 2D:4D, that prenatal androgenization
affects this variable. It is thus important to clarify the relationship between 2D:4D and adult
circulating sex hormone levels, which is our objective.
Is it plausible to assume a correlation between 2D:4D and adult sex hormone titres? A
causal effect of adult sex hormones on 2D:4D is unlikely because the onset of puberty does not
change the sex difference in 2D:4D (Manning et al., 1998; McIntyre et al., 2005; Trivers et al.,
2006). Nonetheless, theoretical considerations make an association plausible. This is, because
prenatal exposure to sex hormones, affecting 2D:4D, may be associated with adult circulating sex
hormone levels (Figure 1c). Prenatal T appears to stem from the fetuses and not from their
mothers. This is indicated by the fact that amniotic T levels are not correlated with maternal
serum T levels (van de Beek et al, 2004). But if prenatal and adult T stem from the same organs
their titres may show a positive correlation. This should effect an association between 2D:4D and
adult levels of circulating sex hormones (Figure 1c).
So far, we have focused on a potential association between adult T levels and 2D:4D. For
three reasons, however, it is necessary to broaden the scope to other sex hormones. First, 2D:4D
may not exclusively reflect prenatal T levels but also prenatal estradiol (Lutchmaya et al, 2004).
Second, complex relationships between sex hormones are common. And third, empirical findings
suggest an association of 2D:4D with adult FSH and LH levels (see below).
We now turn to the empirical evidence regarding potential associations between 2D:4D
and adult sex hormones. Indirect evidence supports the idea of an association between 2D:4D
and adult T. Manning et al. (2002) found a negative relationship between parents' 2D:4D and
offspring sex ratio (i.e., proportion of sons). Because parents' current T levels at the time of
conception are positively related to sex ratio, this result suggests a negative association between
2D:4D and adult circulating T levels. Previous direct findings regarding associations between
2D:4D and adult sex hormone levels are listed in detail in Tables 3 and 4. In brief, these findings
are incomplete and inconsistent. Relationships between 2D:4D and adult T have been studied
best. Conflicting results have been obtained for women (e.g., van Anders and Hampson, 2005,
vs. Benderlioglu and Nelson, 2004) and men (e.g., Manning et al., 1998, vs. Bang et al., 2005).
Whereas studies on women have focused exclusively on T, studies on men have obtained (partly
conflicting) results, which suggest an association of 2D:4D with FSH and LH (Manning et al.,
2004, vs. Bang et al. 2005).
We present new data investigating, for the first time, the relationship of 2D:4D with 17-β-
estradiol (E2), FSH, LH, and progesterone in women and with E2 in men. Studying E2 is
especially important because 2D:4D may be related to prenatal estradiol (Lutchmaya et al, 2004);
studying FSH and LH in women is important because of the already mentioned findings in men
(Bang et al. 2005; Manning et al., 2004). We also investigated the association between 2D:4D and
T, because conflicting findings have been obtained before. We then review al studies that have
investigated associations between adult sex hormone levels and 2D:4D, using meta-analytic
techniques to summarize multiple results.
Synthesizing multiple results by meta-analyses has several advantages. First, meta-analyses
have a higher statistical power than the primary studies they are based upon. Thus, they may
reveal effects that tend not to be detected in single studies yield. Moreover, meta-analyses yield
especially precise estimates of the size of such effects. In the present context, this is highly
desirable because primary studies have rarely studied large samples. The statistical power of
primary studies in this field also suffers from the limited reliability of single hormone
measurements. Sex hormone levels tend to vary from day to day, across the day, and they
respond to external events (van Anders and Watson, 2006). Thus, multiple hormone measures
would be desirable to arrive at precise measurements. Probably for economic reasons, this has
not been done in most studies reviewed here (including our own). Second, meta-analyses can
resolve conflicts arising from conflicting primary results. Meta-analyses help to understand
whether conflicting evidence stems from chance, from differences in statistical power, or from
the fact that the studied effect varies across populations (e.g., clinical vs. normal samples) or
across particulars of methods (e.g., time of hormone measurement). Conflicting evidence is likely
due to particulars of samples or methods if effect sizes vary more strongly across studies than
would be expected by chance (Shadish and Haddock, 1994).
Similar to 2D:4D, left hand 2D:4D minus right hand 2D:4D (Dr-l) has also been suggested
to be a negative correlate of prenatal androgenization in humans (Manning, 2002a; Manning et
al., 2003). As Dr-l is sometimes used as a putative negative measure of prenatal androgenization
(e.g., Manning, 2002a) we extend our present analyses to relationships between Dr-l and adult
circulating sex hormone levels.
2. New data
2.1. Methods
The original female sample (Sample 1) consisted of 66 women who were recruited via newspaper
advertisements, leaflets, and newsgroup postings in Chemnitz, Dresden, and Leipzig (Saxony,
Germany). All women gave their informed consent to participating in a larger study on sex
hormones and attractiveness. Only women who did not use hormonal contraceptives were
eligible to participate. Al women were nulliparous and happened to be Caucasian. One woman
was dropped from the original sample because her finger lengths were not measured; another
participant was dropped because her sex hormones could not be measured within the intended
time frame (day four to ten after menstruation onset). The male sample (Sample 2) consisted of
102 men from Chemnitz and vicinity who participated in a study on physical fitness and
attractiveness (Hönekopp et al., in press). For a large subgroup of the male sample, Hönekopp et
al. (2006a) previously reported on the association between 2D:4D and sexual behavior, and
Hönekopp et al. (2006b) previously reported on the association between 2D:4D and physical
Between 0800h and 1000h, 5 ml venous blood was taken from all participants. For
women, this took place four to ten days after menstruation onset. We chose this time frame
because sex hormone levels remain relatively stable across this part of the menstrual cycle. Serum
was obtained, aliquoted, and stored at -20°C. Economic constraints caused the use of different
methods of analysis for both samples. Electrochemiluminescence immunoassays Elecsys 17-β-
estradiol, progesterone, LH, FSH (Roche Diagnostics GmbH, Mannheim, Germany) and LIA
ACCESS Testosterone (Beckman Coulter Inc., Fullerton CA, USA) were used to analyze the
women's samples. The kits show inter-assay variabilities < 13% (E2 and T), < 12%
(progesterone), < 10% (SHBG), and < 5% (LH and FSH). Radioimmunoassay was used to
analyze the men's samples for E2, T, and SHBG. The kits RIA ACTIVETM TESTOSTERONE,
RIA SHBG, and RIA ESTRADIOL ULTRA-SENSITIVE (Diagnostic System Laboratories,
Webster TX) were used. These kits show inter-assay variabilities <10%, <5%, and <12%,
respectively. All samples from women were analyzed together; the same was true for all samples
from men. All analyzes were performed at the Klinikum Chemnitz, following the manufacturer's
protocol. All control values were within the range specified by the manufacturer.
For the men, we computed a free androgen index (FAI) as total T/SHBG, which is highly
correlated with bioavailable T (Nanjee and Wheeler, 1985).
Participants' finger lengths were measured from digital photographs of the palmar
surfaces of both hands. Participants lightly pressed their palms against a glass plate
perpendicularly mounted on a table. Pictures were then taken through the glass plate. Three
people blind to all other data independently measured the finger lengths using morph man 2.01
software, which allows storing the coordinates of specified pixels. Men's 2D:4D were taken from
Hönekopp et al. (2006b), who successfully used this method for measuring 2D:4D.
2.2. Results
2D:4D measurements proved to be highly reliable for women's right hands (Cronbach's
alpha = .93) and women's left hands (alpha = .94). Accordingly, we averaged, for each hand, all
three 2D:4D measurements. All analyses are based on these averages.
Descriptive statistics for the sample of 64 women are listed in Table 1. As expected, none
of the hormone measurements were correlated with the time span between the onset of
menstruation and serum extraction (all unsigned
r < .08, all
P > .54). Descriptive statistics for the
sample of 102 men are also listed in Table 1.
In women, 2D:4D in the right and in the left hand were correlated
r = .70 (
P < .001); in
men, this correlation was
r = .68 (
P < .001). As expected, women's as compared to men's
2D:4Ds were larger in the right hand (independent-samples
t test:
t164 = 3.6,
P < .001) and in the
left hand (
t164 = 2.9,
P = .005). In line with Putz et al. (2004), Dr-l did not differ significantly
between the sexes (
t164 = 0.5,
P = .48).
The correlations between digit ratios and sex hormones are listed in Table 2 for both
samples. None of them proved statistically significant. This includes LH and FSH in women,
which have been found to be positively associated with 2D:4D in men before (e.g., Manning et
3. Meta-analytic review
We next review al findings on relationships between 2D:4D and adult levels of
circulating sex hormones that are available at present. If more than one study addressed a
particular relationship, we integrated the multiple study results using meta-analytic techniques.
3.1. Method
We used the data bases PsycINFO and Entrez PubMed to find relevant studies, using the
search terms
2D:4D,
finger length*, and
digit ratio. We used the reference section in the retrieved
documents to look for further studies pertaining to our issue. All studies that reported
relationships between 2D:4D and adult levels of circulating sex hormones were included in our
review. Our analyses were based on 10 documents reporting the results of 15 samples, plus
Samples 1 and 2 reported above. Altogether, these 17 samples comprised 332 female and 850
male participants.
Relationships between 2D:4D and circulating levels of sex hormones may differ between
women and men. Also, potential associations may depend on the hand from which 2D:4D is
being measured (Manning, 2002a). We therefore analyzed all data separately for women and men,
and for right hand 2D:4D, left hand 2D:4D, and Dr-l. Studies that analyzed T from saliva and FAI
from serum were analyzed together because both methods measure bioavailable T.
Most studies provided the relevant effect sizes (i.e., correlations) directly. In some cases,
we had to compute them from test statistics, which we did as suggested by Rosenthal (1994). If
studies reported non-significant correlations but did not give the precise values, we wrote to the
authors, who kindly provided the exact results. In all cases, the analyzed effect sizes were zero-
order correlations. If more than one study reported a specific relationship, we computed the
mean effect size from
z-transformed correlations, weighting each effect size according to its
sample size (i.e., with
n-3). For each mean weighted effect size, we computed the lower and upper
bound of the .95-confidence interval. We tested if the primary results varied more than would be
expected by chance (Shadish & Haddock, 1994). Significantly heterogeneous results indicate that
the studied effect depends on the population studied, on the method employed, or on some
other systematic factor. If between-study variance was greater than expected by chance (
P ≤.05),
we used a random effects model to compute the confidence interval, following Equation 18-23
from Shadish and Haddock (1994). In this case, we also computed
SDrnd, which indicates how
strongly the studied effect varies across populations, methods of study, or other systematic
factors. If the results of the primary studies were homogeneous (i.e., varied within the limits
expected by chance) the computation of the confidence interval followed a fixed effects model.
3.2. Results and discussion
We first turn to those associations between 2D:4D and adult sex hormones that have
been studied in more than one sample. The primary findings are listed in the left part of Table 3.
Its right part shows the aggregated results. In general, associations between sex hormones and
digit ratios proved to be non-significant and the variance in results across studies was not greater
than expected by chance. However, three statistically significant exceptions are noteworthy, all of
which pertain to men. Dr-l showed a low negative correlation with total T; right hand 2D:4D
showed a small positive correlation with FSH; and results regarding right hand 2D:4D and LH
showed substantial between-studies variance.
We next turn to those associations that have been studied in only one sample (Table 4).
As can be seen, non-significant results (most of them stemming from our Samples 1 and 2)
prevail. Two noteworthy exceptions stem from Manning et al. (2004), who found substantial
associations of left hand 2D:4D with FSH and LH in men.
In sum, Tables 3 and 4 contain five unusual (i.e., statistically significant) results. It is
noteworthy that all five exceptions are partly or entirely based on a highly atypical sample of
infertile men, many of which probably had severely compromised testicular function (Manning et
al., 2004). Three of the five exceptional findings involved at least one other sample; in all three
cases, the sample of infertile men produced the most extreme result. When we omitted the results
from this atypical sample from our analyses, the relationship between Dr-l and total T from serum
in men was not statistically significant any more (mean weighted
r = -.13,
rlower = -.28,
rupper = .03).
Thus, only one out of five unusual results remained significant (Table 3, figure in bold face):
Bang et al. (2005) found a small correlation between FSH and right hand 2D:4D in normal men.
Given the high number (29) of remaining associations between 2D:4D or Dr-l and levels of
circulating sex hormones that have been studied a single significant result is about what one
would expect by chance.
Manning et al. (2004) conclude from their findings that the variability in circulating T
levels among male patients of infertility clinics may be greater than in normal controls. An
unusually high variance in circulating T may explain why there appears to be a relationship with
2D:4D in this sub-population.
4. General Discussion
The length ratio of the second (index) to the fourth (ring) finger (2D:4D), a putative
negative correlate of prenatal T, has become a popular variable for studying effects of prenatal
androgenization in humans. However, 2D:4D may not exclusively reflect prenatal sex hormones
but also adult circulating sex hormone levels. Therefore, observed relations between 2D:4D and
variables of interest in adults may not reflect effects of prenatal androgenization, as is commonly
assumed, but the effects of circulating sex hormones. Here, we set out to clarify the relationship
between 2D:4D and adult sex hormone levels. We did so by providing new data from two non-
clinical samples (comprising of 64 women and 102 men) and by reviewing and meta-analyzing the
available evidence.
Our review pertaines to 32 relationships between putative indicators of prenatal
androgenization (left hand 2D:4D, right hand 2D:4D, Dr-l) and measures of circulating levels of
sex hormones (total T, bioavailable T, E2, LH, FSH, and progesterone). Several significant
relationships, involving T, FSH, and LH, were obtained for men. However, these relationships
depended largely or entirely on one highly atypical sample of infertile men (Manning et al., 2004).
Without the results from this sample, we obtained data on 29 relationships. Only one of them,
pertaining to right hand 2D:4D and FSH in men, was statistically significant. For a number of
reasons, this finding does not challenge the usefulness of 2D:4D as an indicator of prenatal
androgenization. First, given the large number of associations examined, this finding may well
have arisen from chance. Second, although statistically significant, the relationship was very small
(
r = .12). Third, and most importantly, considerations regarding the effects of sex hormones on
behavior and cognition focus on T in males and on T, estrogen, and progesterone in females
(e.g., Fitch and Denenberg, 1998; Keefe, 2002). We are not aware of results or considerations
that suggest an effect of current FSH levels on men's behavior or cognition. For the theoretically
more important hormones, T, estrogen, and progesterone, the present evidence does not suggest
an association with 2D:4D or Dr-l in the normal population.
A word of caution is in order regarding the relationship between 2D:4D and circulating T
levels in men. Recently, van den Bergh and Dewitte (2006) found that 2D:4D predicts T spikes
triggered by sexual cues in men. Thus, researchers should ensure that obtained relationships
between 2D:4D and actual behavior do not reflect effects of a transient T spike (e.g. caused by
the presence of an attractive experimenter) on that behavior.
Our review was entirely based on published studies. The results of published studies may
not be representative for the results of all studies that have investigated the particular question of
interest. This is because statistically significant studies as compared to statistically non significant
studies have a higher chance to get published. The resulting underrepresentation of non
significant results is a potential threat to the validity of meta-analyses (e.g., Begg, 1994). This is
not true in our case because unpublished, non significant results would strengthen but not
weaken our conclusions.
However, one should keep in mind that the absence of evidence is not the same as the
evidence of absence. Those of our results that did not pertain to T were often based on only one
sample, and the respective confidence intervals were often large. Thus, we cannot rule out that
future studies may reveal moderate associations between 2D:4D or Dr-l and levels of circulating
sex hormones in the normal population. However, the present evidence suggests that neither
2D:4D nor Dr-l are associated with adults' sex hormone levels in healthy adults. This supports the
validity of 2D:4D as a means to study the effects of prenatal androgenization on human behavior
and cognition.
We would like to thank al participants. We also thank Nina Asperger, Astrid Lang, Ute
Lausmann, and Anja Miethe for help with the collection of data. We are grateful to Georg Jahn
and Udo Rudolph for helpful comments on an earlier draft. This research was supported by
Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft grants HO 2506/1-1 and HO 2506/3-1 to J.H.
Alexander, G. M., 2006. Associations among gender-linked toy preferences, digit ratio, and spatial
ability: evidence from eye-tracking analysis. Arch. Sex. Behav. 35, 699-709.
Bang, A.K., Carlsen, E., Holm, M., Petersen, J.H., Skakkebaek, N.E., Jørgensen, N., 2005. A
study of finger lengths, semen quality and sex hormones in 360 young men from the
general Danish population. Hum. Reprod. 20, 3109-3113.
Begg, C.B., 1994. Publication bias. In: H. Cooper and L.V. Hedges (Eds.),
The Handbook of
Research Synthesis. Russell Sage, New York, pp. 399-410.
Benderlioglu, Z., Nelson, R.J., 2004. Digit length ratios predict reactive aggression in women, but
not in men. Horm. Behav. 46, 558-564.
Brown, W. M., Hines, M., Fane, B. A., Breedlove S.M., 2002. Masculinized finger length patterns
in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm. Behav. 42, 380-
Buck, J.J., Williams, R.M., Hughes, I.A., Acerini, C.L., 2003. In utero exposure and 2nd to 4th
digit length ratio. Comparisons between healthy controls and females with classical
congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Hum. Reprod. 18, 976-979.
Burton, L.A., Henninger, D., Hafetz, J., 2005. Gender differences in relations of mental rotation,
verbal fluency, and SAT scores to finger length ratios as hormonal indexes. Dev.
Neuropsychol. 28, 493-505.
Cohen-Bendahan, C.C.C., van de Beek, C., Berenbaum, S.A., 2005. Prenatal sex hormone effects
on child and adult sex- typed behavior: Methods and findings. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
29, 353-384.
Collaer, M.L., Hines, M., 1995. Human behavioral sex differences: a role for gonadal hormones
during early development? Psych. Bull. 118, 55-107.
Coolican, J., Peters, M., 2003. Sexual dimorphism in the 2D/4D ratio and its relation to mental
rotation performance. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 179-183.
Falter, C.M., Arroyo, M., Davis, G.J., 2006. Testosterone: activation or organization of spatial
cognition? Biol. Psychol. 73, 132-140.
Fitch, R.H., Denenberg, V.H., 1998. A role of ovarian hormones in sexual differentiation of the
brain. Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 311-352.
Hönekopp, J., Manning, J.T., Müller, C., 2006b. Digit ratio (2D:4D) and physical
fitness in males and females: evidence for effects of prenatal androgens on sexually
selected traits. Horm. Behav. 49, 545-549.
Hönekopp, J., Rudolph, U., Beier, L., Liebert, A., Müller, C., in press. Physical attractiveness of
face and body as indicators of physical fitness. Evol. Hum. Behav.
Hönekopp, J., Voracek, M., Manning, J.T., 2006a. 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) and
number of sex partners: evidence for effects of prenatal testosterone in men.
PNEC 31, 30-37.
Keefe, D.L., 2002. Sex hormones and neural mechanisms. Arch. Sex. Behav. 31, 401-403.
Kempel, P., Gohlke, B., Klempau, J., Zinsberger, P., Reuter, M., Hennig, J., 2005. Second-to-
fourth digit length, testosterone and spatial ability. Intelligence 33, 215-230.
Klump, K.L., Gobrogge, K.L., Perkins, P.S., Thorne, D., Sisk, C.L., Breedlove, S.M., 2006.
Preliminary evidence that gonadal hormones organize and activate disordered eating.
Psychol. Med. 36, 539-546.
Knickmeyer, R.C., Wheelwright, S., Taylor, K., Raggatt, P., Hackett, G., Baron-Cohen, S. 2005.
Gender-typed play and amniotic testosterone. Dev. Psychol. 41, 517-528.
Kondo, T., Zákány, J., Innis, J. W., Duboule, D., 1997. Of fingers, toes, and penises. Nature 390,
Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., Knickmeyer, R., Manning, J.T., 2004. 2nd to
4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol. Early Hum. Dev. 77, 23-28.
Malas, M.A., Dogan, S., Evcil, E.H., Desdicioglu, K., 2006. Fetal development of the hand, digits
and digit ratio (2D:4D). Early Hum. Dev. 82, 469-475.
Manning, J.T., 2002a. Digit ratio: a pointer to fertility, behavior, and health. Rutgers University
Press, New Brunswick.
Manning, J.T., 2002b. The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length and performance in skiing. J. Sports
Med. Phys. Fitness 42, 446-450.
Manning, J.T., Barley, L., Walton, J., Lewis-Jones, D.I., Trivers, R.L., Singh, D., Thornhill, R.,
Rhode, P., Bereczkei, T., Henzi, P., Soler, M., Szwed, A., 2000. The 2nd:4th digit ratio,
sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success: Evidence for
sexually antagonistic genes? Evol. Hum. Behav. 21, 163-183.
Manning, J.T., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Sanders, G., 2001. The 2nd to 4th digit ratio
and autism. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 160-164.
Manning, J.T., Bundred, P.E., Newton, D.J., Flanagan, B.F., 2003. The second to fourth digit
ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene. Evol. Hum. Behav. 24, 399-405.
Manning, J.T., Martin, S., Trivers, R.L., Soler, M., 2002. 2nd to 4th digit ratio and offspring sex
ratio. J. Theor. Biol. 217, 93-95.
Manning, J.T., Scutt, D., Wilson, J., Lewis-Jones, D.I., 1998. The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length:
a predictor of sperm numbers and levels of testosterone, LH and oestrogen. Hum.
Reprod. 13, 3000-3004.
Manning, J.T, Wood, S., Vang, E., Walton, J., Bundred, P.E., van Heyningen, C., Lewis-Jones
D.I., 2004. Second to fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) and testosterone in men. Asian J Androl.
McIntyre, M.H., 2006. The use of digit ratios as markers for prenatal androgen action. Reprod.
Biol. Endocrin., 4:10.
McIntyre, M.H., Ellison, P.T., Lieberman, D.E., Demerath, E., Towne, B., 2005. The
development of sex differences in digital formula from infancy in the Fels Longitudinal
Study. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 272, 1473-1479.
Nanjee, N.M., Wheeler, M.J., 1985. Plasma free testosterone – is an index sufficient? Annals of
Clin. Biochem. 22, 387-390.
Neave, N., Laing, S., Fink, B., Manning, J.T., 2003. Second to fourth digit ratio, testosterone and
perceived male dominance. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270, 2167-2172.
Neave, N., Wolfson, S., 2003. Testosterone, territoriality, and the ‘home advantage'. Physiol.
Behav. 78, 269-275.
Ökten, A., Kalyoncu, M. Yaris, N., 2002. The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and
congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency. Early Hum. Dev. 70, 47-
Paul, S.N., Kato, B.S., Hunkin, J.L., Vivekanandan, S., Spector, T.D., 2006. The Big Finger: the
second to fourth digit ratio is a predictor of sporting ability in women. Brit. J. Sports
Med. 40, 981-983.
Pokrywka, L., Rachon, D., Suchecka-Rachon, K., Bitel, L., 2005. The second to fourth digit ratio
in elite and non-elite female athletes. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 17, 796-800.
Putz, D.A., Gaulin, S.J.C., Sporter, R.J., McBurney, D.H., 2004. Sex hormones and finger length.
What does 2D:4D indicate? Evol. Hum. Behav. 25, 182-199.
Roney, J.R., Maestripieri, D., 2004. Relative digit lengths predict men's behaviour and
attractiveness during social interactions with women. Hum. Nature 15, 271-282.
Rosenthal, R., 1994. Parametric measures of effect size. In: H. Cooper and L.V. Hedges (Eds.),
The Handbook of Research Synthesis. Russell Sage, New York, pp. 231-244.
Sanders, G., Bereczkei, T., Csatho, A., Manning, J., 2005. The ratio of the 2nd to 4th finger
length predicts spatial ability in men but not women. Cortex 41, 789-95.
Shadish, W.R., Haddock, C.K., 1994. Combining estimates of effect sizes. In: H. Cooper and
L.V. Hedges (Eds.), The Handbook of Research Synthesis. Russell Sage, New York, pp.
Trivers, R., Manning, J, Jacobson, A., 2006. A longitudinal study of digit ratio (2D:4D) and other
finger ratios in Jamaican children. Horm. Behav. 49, 150-156.
van Anders, S.M., Hampson, E., 2005. Testing the prenatal androgen hypothesis: measuring digit
ratios, sexual orientation, and spatial abilities in adults. Horm. Behav. 47, 92-98.
van Anders, S.M., Watson, N.V., 2006. Social neuroendocrinology. Effects of social contexts and
behaviors on sex steroids in humans. Hum. Nature 17, 212-237.
van Anders, S.M., Vernon, P.A., Wilbur, C.J., 2006. Finger-length ratios show evidence of
prenatal hormone-transfer between opposite-sex twins. Horm. Behav. 49, 315-319.
van de Beek, C., Thijssen, J.H.H., Cohen-Kettenis, P.T., van Goozen, S.H.M., Buitelaar, J.K.,
2004. Relationships between sex hormones assessed in amniotic fluid, and maternal and
umbilical cord serum: what is the best source of information to investigate the effects of
fetal hormonal exposure? Horm. and Behav. 46, 663-669.
van den Bergh, B., Dewitte, S., 2006. Digit ratio (2D:4D) moderates the impact of sexual cues on
men's decisions in ultimatum games. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 273, 2091–2095.
Figure 1: How should a correlation between 2D:4D and the study variable (X) be interpreted?
Usually it is seen as indicating an effect of prenatal testosterone on X (a)
. However, given a
correlation between 2D:4D and adult steroid levels, the same correlation may instead indicate
unidirectional or bidirectional causation between adult hormone levels and X (b). The critical
correlation between 2D:4D and adult steroids may arise from an association between foetal T
and adult steroids (c). (Dotted lines indicate correlations, arrows indicate causation).
Table 1: Descriptive statistics for Sample 1 and 2 (M±SD).
Progesteron (nmol/l)
Table 2: Correlations between digit ratios and sex hormones.
Note: r2D:4D = right hand 2D:4D; l2D:4D = left hand 2D:4D. All
P ≥.13.
Table 3. Associations between digit ratios and levels of circulating sex hormones in adults
(multiple samples).
Aggregated findings
Bang et al. (2005)
Benderlioglu and Nelson (2004)
Neave et al. (2003)
Roney and Maestripieri (2004)
Falter et al. (2006)
Kempel et al. (2005)
Benderlioglu and Nelson (2004)
Neave et al. (2003)
Kempel et al. (2005)
Benderlioglu and Nelson (2004)
Benderlioglu and Nelson (2004)
van Anders et al. (2005)
Kallai et al. (2005)
Falter et al. (2006)
Kempel et al. (2005)
van Anders et al. (2005)
Benderlioglu and Nelson (2004)
van Anders et al. (2005)
van Anders et al. (2005)
Kallai et al. (2005)
Kempel et al. (2005)
(Table 3 continues)
Aggregated findings
total testosterone
Bang et al. (2005)
Manning et al. (1998)
34.1±0.7 clinical
Manning et al. (2004)
Manning et al. (2004)
39.0±7.6 clinical
Manning et al. (1998)
34.1±0.7 clinical
Manning et al. (2004)
Manning et al. (2004)
39.0±7.6 clinical
Manning et al. (2004)
Manning et al. (2004)
39.0±7.6 clinical
Bang et al. (2005)
Manning et al. (2004)
39.0±7.6 clinical
Bang et al. (2005)
Manning et al. (2004)
39.0±7.6 clinical
Note:
SD = unaccounted variability in effect sizes between studies; mean
r = weighted mean
effect size;
r
= lower/upper bound of .95 confidence interval; r2D:4D = right hand
2D:4D; l2D:4D = left hand 2D:4D; a = data obtained through personal communication.
Exceptional (i.e., statistically significant) results are underlined. All involve one highly atypical
sample from Manning et al. (2004). The only result that remained statistically
significant after removing the highly atypical sample is printed in bold face.
Table 4. Associations between digit ratios and levels of circulating sex hormones in adults (single
Manning et al. (2004)
Manning et al. (2004)
Manning et al. (2004)
Manning et al. (2004)
total testosterone
Benderlioglu et al. (2004)
17-β-estradiol from serum
progesterone from serum
Note: r2D:4D = right hand 2D:4D; l2D:4D = left hand 2D:4D; a = data obtained through
personal communication. Both statistically significant results (underlined) stem from the same
atypical sample.
Source: https://www.tu-chemnitz.de/hsw/psychologie/professuren/allpsy2/Artikel/PNEC_in_press.pdf
Epidemiological Alert Neurological syndrome, congenital malformations, and Zika virus infection. Implications for public health in the Americas 1 December 2015 Given the increase of congenital anomalies, Guil ain-Barré syndrome, and other neurological and autoimmune syndromes in areas where Zika virus is circulating and their possible relation to the virus, the Pan American Health Organization / World Health Organization (PAHO/WHO) recommends its Member States establish and maintain the capacity to detect and confirm Zika virus cases, prepare healthcare facilities for the possible increase in demand at al healthcare levels and specialized care for neurological syndromes, and to strengthen antenatal care. In addition, Member States should continue efforts to reduce the presence of mosquito vectors through an effective vector control strategy and public communication.
Table of contents Farter, pourquoi donc? Sciolinatura, perché? Testing / Nordic know-how Selbst wer zum ersten Mal auf Skiern steht, eine Même le novice qui, pour la première fois, chausse Persino chi per la prima volta si trova sugli sci, si Loipe betritt oder sich beim Snowboarden versucht – des skis, affronte une piste de ski de fond ou s‘essaie avventura su una pista di fondo o si mette su uno