Doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.01.014
Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
Contents lists available at
Journal of Anxiety Disorders
The use of virtual reality in acrophobia research and treatment
Carlos M. Coelho ,Allison M. Waters , Trevor J. Hine Guy Wallis
a School of Human Movement Studies, University of Queensland, Level 5, Building 26, St. Lucia, QLD 4072, Australiab Griffith Institute of Health and Medical Research and School of Psychology, Griffith University, Brisbane, Australiac Queensland Brain Institute, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
Acrophobia, or fear of heights, is a widespread and debilitating anxiety disorder affecting perhaps 1 in 20
Received 11 August 2008
adults. Virtual reality (VR) technology has been used in the psychological treatment of acrophobia since
Received in revised form 22 January 2009
1995, and has come to dominate the treatment of numerous anxiety disorders. It is now known that
Accepted 29 January 2009
virtual reality exposure therapy (VRET) regimens are highly effective for acrophobia treatment. Thispaper reviews current theoretical understanding of acrophobia as well as the evolution of its common
treatments from the traditional exposure therapies to the most recent virtually guided ones. In
particular, the review focuses on recent innovations in the use of VR technology and discusses the
benefits it may offer for examining the underlying causes of the disorder, allowing for the systematic
assessment of interrelated factors such as the visual, vestibular and postural control systems.
ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Contributing factors to the development and maintenance of acrophobia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Self-locomotion and the role of the visual and vestibular systems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Fear of heights treatment models—an historical perspective . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Acrophobia therapy efficacy: early theory-based approaches . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
1. Introduction and overview of acrophobia
naturalistic type. Although long since recognized as a disorder,there is still some doubt about its typology due to its similarities to
According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
both panic disorder and
Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) ), acrophobia, which
agoraphobia Acrophobia
is an extreme fear of heights, is considered a specific phobia of
appears closely related to the fear of elevators and fear of flying,both of which belong to the specific phobia, situational type, asdefined in the DSM-IV ().
Acrophobic behavior typically involves the avoidance of a variety
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: (C.M. Coelho).
of height-related situations, including stairs, terraces, apartments
0887-6185/$ – see front matter ß 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
and offices located in high buildings, bridges, elevators and plane
Of course, failure to remember any particular experience
trips. Considering the striking breadth of aversive situations and
related to the emergence of the disturbance might equally be
stimuli, it is not surprising that individuals with acrophobia feel
attributed to problems inherent in self-report and memory rather
impaired and restricted in their movements, even in comparison to
than a non-associative etiology (see
sufferers of other specific phobias
). To overcome these methodological problems,
Specific phobias have a high prevalence rate in epidemiological
data, both in adolescents () and
longitudinal study of a large cohort of approximately 1000
in adults (In the ECA Study, which was
participants from birth. Corroborating the previous findings, the
comprised of 20,000 participants, 4.7% of the participants fulfilled
authors noticed that serious falls that resulted in fracture,
the criteria for a diagnosis of acrophobia ). A study
dislocation, laceration or intracranial injury had no positive
by presented similar
correlation with fear of heights in later ages. Contrary to what
results with 6.3% of men and 8.6% of women presenting with
was expected, falls resulting in serious injuries between the ages of
acrophobia. Moreover, this disorder tends to evolve to chronicity
five and nine occurred more frequently in participants who later
largely due to the pervasive avoidance of a wide-
exhibited no discernable fear of heights at the age of 18. Also, no
range of height-related situations that form part of everyday living.
participant with fear of heights at 18 possessed a history of serious
It is clear then, that due to its high prevalence, chronicity, and
falls before nine years of age. In summary, ‘participants with less
cumulative social impact, research into the underlying causes and
fear of heights seem to be those who sustained more injuries due to
treatment of acrophobia is particularly important. Additionally, by
falling and the conditioning experiences do not seem to have
studying underlying acrophobic fear mechanisms, uncertainties
produced fear. .' p. 135). Similar results
can be addressed relating to its typology as well as etiology and
were reported by namely that
treatment. This review makes particular reference to the use of
participants without fear of heights reported a higher incidence
virtual reality (VR) technology as a superior methodology in
of fearful and painful experiences than those with fear, suggesting
achieving improvements in both the treatment and our under-
that those without fear are likely to pay for their innate lack of
standing of the causes of this disorder.
Longitudinal studies of this type do offer some insight into the
2. Contributing factors to the development and maintenance of
processes involved in fear of height acquisition but one might still
arguer that failures in recall may lead to underreporting ofsignificant events. Several studies suggest that memory reliability
This section reviews the available evidence on contributing
decays in the space of weeks or months (
factors to the development and maintenance of acrophobia. To
; ). Perhaps more
date, this literature has tended to focus either on factors gleaned
significant, is the possibility that phobia emerges through
from the broader literature on specific phobias or on the role of
accumulation of subtle, non-traumatic experiences which are
specific factors in isolation, rather than on the development of a
not in themselves very memorable (e.g.
comprehensive model of acrophobia fear acquisition per se.
; ) but that cannonetheless influence long-term behaviors (
2.1. Non-associative accounts
; and give rise to fear ) that is difficult to associate with conditioning.
Because some individuals who fear heights have often been
In general, all of these arguments undermine non-associative
unable to report a clear height-related aversive experience as a
theories of phobias but accord with acrophobia phenomenology. In
primary etiological factor, some authors have proposed hereditary
particular, a fear of heights is acquired before verbal acquisition, at
or non-associative accounts in the disorder's development
a time when children are learning to crawl and falls occur,
educating children about the nature of surfaces
compared fearful and non-fearful participants' responses to heights
locomotion, balance and posture (). Whilst
in an attempt to assess their acquisition of height-related fear.
Adolph noted that learning strategies do not transfer from crawling
Results showed that 21 participants were categorized as developing
to walking, the avoidance of heights does seem to be maintained
their fears through non-associative pathways. Additionally, the non-
across developmental postures
fearful group also had participants who had experienced direct
). It is therefore plausible that some of these
conditioning relevant events but did not develop a fear of heights.
skills act as latent inhibition, thus preventing more skilful children
These data were seen as consistent with a non-associative account
from developing a fear of heights.
for the acquisition of the fear of heights, in which ontogeneticlearning is not required, at least for the fear of heights.
2.2. Role of cognitive processes
A few years later, were able to
extend these findings using a much larger clinically defined group
Combining cognitive and biological factors,
of acrophobics (n = 148) who were age and sex-matched to a non-
argued that fear of heights may develop in ways similar to panic
phobic control group. The phobic group met the DSM-III-R criteria
disorder. That is, cognitive biases may develop leading individuals
for simple phobia ) and had a mean age of 40.3 years.
to increasingly interpret bodily sensations that are tied to
The results of this study questioned once again the significance of
movement in height-related situations as threatening. These
associative-learning events in the acquisition of fear of heights,
researchers assessed 100 students for bodily sensations, acropho-
since the categories consistent with the ethological theory
bia, spider phobia, cognitive bias, trait anxiety, and depression
accounted for 83 participants. Moreover, the authors did not find
using a suite of questionnaires. Analysis revealed a correlation
significant differences between groups with respect to the
between measures of acrophobia and a bias to interpret and report
proportion of participants who exhibited other height-related
internal bodily sensations of anxiety as threatening. There was no
fears, or who experienced relevant associative-learning events. The
such bias evident for external stimuli or social stimuli and the bias
ages at which these events had occurred were also not significantly
was not found with measures of spider phobia. The authors
different. All this evidence led the authors to rule out a latent
concluded that ‘in the case of the development of acrophobia,
inhibition hypothesis (e.g.
individuals may come to interpret some bodily sensations as
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
indicative of dizziness and nausea, and these may occur in
) as well as inherited factors, such as preparedness
situational circumstances which might lead the individual to
(e.g. ). If the majority of humans have an
associate these signs with the possibility of an imminent
inherited disposition to avoid heights and this avoidance can later
catastrophic fall (e.g. if they are looking down a circular stairway,
develop into a phobia, it is equally possible that people with low
or standing on a chair)' p. 1000). Thus,
levels of fear develop an absence of fear through habituation that
conclusion highlights the possibility that
develops from frequent exposure opportunities. Conversely, those
cognitive and biological vulnerabilities can underlie the develop-
born with a particular fear can fail to overcome it due to lack of safe
ment of height fear in humans and that a catastrophic, condition-
exposure in early life (see
ing event may not be necessary.
The importance of each vulnerability and learning factor
2.3. Contemporary learning models
remains a matter of ongoing debate. What is clear from studiesin children (e.g. ;
Davey et al.'s integrated approach is echoed in the work of
is that a fear of heights accompanies the emergence of
who emphasized the need to recognize
self locomotion (crawling) and that habituation processes can aid
the concept of preparedness, that is that certain stimuli are
in overcoming a fear of heights.
evolutionally predisposed to evoke fear responses (This, they argued, could help answer some of the questions
2.4. Self-locomotion and the role of the visual and vestibular systems
that classical conditioning accounts could not. In particular, theyargued that it was necessary to provide an explanation for the fact
One of the earliest laboratory-based investigations of fear of
that not all fearful participants have a history of failed learning
heights was based on studies of the ‘visual cliff'—a transparent
experiences, while at the same time some participants without fear
walkway spanning a drop. These studies were conducted with a
have been through traumatic experiences ). From
variety of animals (e.g. and human
their perspective, and in contrast to non-associative accounts (see
a diathetic-stress model offers
the most attractive framework for describing acrophobia, since it
). The animals and humans
considers learning histories (such as vicarious and modeling
showed various reactions to the cliff, each of which was
learning) within the context of inherited ‘prepared' liabilities, such
characteristic of the particular species studied. For example, in
as the personality trait of neuroticism, for example. Together, these
human infants, the studies suggested a developmental shift
vulnerabilities make certain individuals more or less susceptible to
corresponding to a marked increase in a fear of heights after the
developing anxiety disorders (
onset of crawling ). Although some initial
degree of fear of heights is a normative experience of development,
What has yet to emerge from this more holistic, multifaceted
most individuals overcome this fear through processes inherent to
approach is a description of how the various factors interact.
development itself – practice, exposure, development of mastery,
Combining learning and biological factors appears to suggest that
etc. – to eventually retain a degree of healthy wariness about
the development of acrophobic depends on a combination of
heights that is functional and adaptive to survival. At present, it is
specific and general factors including, for example, cognitive
unclear if acrophobia is in someway related to a failure to achieve
biases, the extent of prior experience with heights, and possibly,
this developmental milestone.
the development of a sense of control over the height environment.
recently tested eight participants
Control might therefore be achieved in a learning process similar to
diagnosed with acrophobia in a virtual reality environment and
the one used by in the treatment of the
found that participants experienced elevated anxiety not only to
fear of dogs. They argued that participants who had previous
increases in height, but also when required to move laterally at a
experience with dogs have more knowledge of how dogs behave,
fixed height. These anxiety levels were significantly higher than
and that they may view a dog's behavior as predictable and
those elicited by viewing the fear-evoking scene without move-
controllable, and hence less threatening
ment. Coelho et al.'s results suggested a potential link between
As it will be argued later, participants with acrophobia might tend
movement and acrophobia in line with earlier developmental
to interpret the discrepancies present in elevated
studies of infant crawling, such as the ones of
environments as sign of a possible fall. Through extended
. As children learn to detect threats to balance
experience of visuo-vestibular interactions in elevated locations,
and find compensatory strategies to regain posture when in
the link between this sign and an imminent fall becomes
disequilibrium acrophobic participants might
diminished, and with it, the associated fear.
similarly learn to use the visual, vestibular and somatosensory
Both the non-associative and the diathesis-stress models
systems to learn how to move in height-related situations.
accord with account that experience and
Moreover, similar to motion sickness, fear of heights might be
habituation with the environment can work towards eliminating
due to a conflict between vision, somatosensory and vestibular
biologically relevant fears. In the particular case of heights,
senses This mismatch can occur because of the
habituation is observed in individuals who go through repeated
image flow associated with head movement i.e. motion parallax.
exposure to heights such as ‘steeplejacks, roof-workers, and tight-
Motion parallax is the perspective transformations of the retinal
rope artists, who achieve a remarkable degree of postural balance
image, produced by either the movement of an observer or the
with seeming insensitivity to height' (
movement of objects in the visual world ().
, p. 513). The absence of fear of heights in certain
These transformations are greatly reduced for objects viewed at
individuals can be taken as evidence for both learning processes
large distances ). This issue is addressed later in
such as habituation, latent inhibition and immunization (e.g.
The evidence of the abovementioned research studies suggests
that it is too soon to defend a behavioral, diathesis-stress or non-
associative model for the etiology of fear of heights. It seems that
The conflict exists when the prevailing inputs from the visual and vestibular
some models are in fact quite complementary. Some of the liability
systems are at variance with stored patterns derived from previous interactionswith the spatial environment (see ).
for fear of heights might be inherited, however it is also likely that
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
such a fear might only manifest if other factors: vicarious,
exposure itself. This model gives emphasis to the quality and
informational or behavioral, are part of the individual story.
quantity of information as the main factors to increase the self-efficacy that participants acquire and the manner in which they
3. Fear of heights treatment models—an historical perspective
incorporate that information when assessing their own capacities().
proposed one of the first theoretical accounts of
acrophobia to be used as a basis for treatment. The two-factor
4. Acrophobia therapy efficacy: early theory-based approaches
theory of phobia treatment is still in use today and has remainedpractically unchanged. Fundamentally, this theory states that
The earliest research on acrophobia treatment followed these
phobic anxiety is a conditioned response, triggered by a condi-
early theoretical approaches. For example, exam-
tioned stimulus (the phobic situation). However, it also adds that
ined a ‘‘contact desensitization'' treatment (CD) involving the use
situational avoidance develops and remains due to the reinforce-
of contact with the therapist's hand and arm while engaging in
ment of reduced anxiety (negative reinforcement). This successful
approach responses to the desired behavior. The physical
avoidance of the phobic situation assures the perpetuation of the
assistance was then gradually eliminated with eventual indepen-
anxiety. Currently, it is widely accepted that because avoidance
dent response rehearsal by the participant. Ritter considered the
decreases anxiety in the short-term, it is a major contributor to fear
therapeutically effective components of CD to be: (a) information
regarding the avoided object or situation; (b) observation of
). However, by avoiding
model(s) performing target behaviors with no adverse conse-
and/or escaping their fears quickly, phobic individuals do not
quences; (c) contact with the phobic object; (d) contact with the
experience their distress diminishing in anxiety-provoking situa-
therapist; (e) skill obtained in performing the desired behavior
through rehearsal; (f) hierarchical exposure to the avoided object
Following on from Mower's early work, devel-
or situation and to the desired responses. For testing these
oped a technique called ‘systematic desensitization' that was a
components, the therapist worked with 12 participants (10
milestone in the development of effective phobia treatment.
women and two men) who were extremely fearful of heights.
Wolpe had participants ‘‘avoid'' avoidance, that is, remain
Participants were exposed to one of the following three conditions:
steadfast in the presence of the feared stimulus.
(1) treatment with CD; (2) treatment which involved all elements
explains that this procedure consists of imaginarily linking feared
of CD except for physical contact, or (3) no treatment. The
situations with muscle relaxation, which works as an anxiety
treatment consisted of successive approaches (in groups) to
inhibiting response. Central to the development of this procedure
progressively higher landings of a staircase with nine landings,
was the notion that deep muscular relaxation and the imagination
15.8 m high in total, until two minutes exposure was achieved at
of relaxing scenes was an anxiety antagonist. Wolpe suggested that
the highest landing. The contact between the therapist and the
relaxation could be capable of suppressing the anxiogenic
participant involved placing the therapist's (woman's) arm around
properties of the stimulus, breaking the link between them. A
the waist of each participant and leading him/her to the testing
series of later studies supported the fact that systematic
site. Gradually the therapist removed the arm while the participant
desensitization was effective in eliminating fears and after Wolpe,
was still at the lowest landing, and gradually the therapist
numerous studies have tried to uncover the active therapeutic
extended the period of time the participant stayed at the testing site
without contact. The exposure was performed in groups and part of
Later, self-efficacy theory ) identified the
the treatment involved the participants linking arms and walking to
participants' judgment about their ability to perform specific
the edge of the stairway. Results showed significantly better results
actions as a major cognitive determinant of human behavior.
in the group with physical contact. In another study,
Although self-efficacy judgments and anticipated outcomes jointly
compared CD with the same treatment without contact (demon-
determine behavior, Bandura claimed that phobic behavior is
stration-plus-participation – DP) and modeling. The study involved
influenced more by self-efficacy judgments than by outcome
13 women and two men with severe height phobia. Results revealed
expectations. When people have confidence that they can
that CD treatment was similar to DP and modeling, producing
successfully execute various courses of action, then their
clinically reliable benefits. In a related study,
expectations regarding consequences should tend to be the major
did not observe differences in acrophobia treatment with CD
determinants of performance variation. However, when people
as a function of ‘‘warm'' versus ‘‘cold'' types of therapist behavior.
have strong incentives to perform a given action their perceptions
The warm therapist maintained eye contact, used voice inflection
of self-efficacy will tend to exercise the greater influence over
and facial expression of friendliness and support. The cold therapist
whether they attempt it and how well they succeed (
avoided eye contact and spoke ‘‘nonchalantly'' while maintaining a
blank facial expression.
Several treatment models emerged from this early work, in
compared two types of
particular, ‘‘reinforced practice'' and ‘‘self-efficacy'' treatments.
treatments for acrophobia: systematic desensitization applied by
Reinforced practice is a combination of ‘‘therapeutic ingredients''
the therapist versus the same intervention applied by the
combined in a therapeutic model (and assumes
participant with the help of a tape recorder replaying desensitiza-
that improvements in cognitive and physiological components of
tion instructions. The study, which involved 30 acrophobic
anxiety will occur after modeling approach responses and
participants (nine men and 21 women), revealed that the two
continuous practice of these responses
interventions were equally effective. Interestingly, the only
difference emerged in a later follow-up test after eight months,
The therapeutic ingredients
which revealed a greater improvement for participants who used
included exposure to stimuli that trigger fear, therapeutic
the tape recorder. The results suggest that desensitization is an
instructions, client progress control, feedback on performance,
effective treatment even with reduced therapeutic contact. The
and conditional performance reinforcement
authors concluded that some participants functioned better in a
). In contrast, self-efficacy treatment places more
therapeutic situation that offered greater autonomy and control
emphasis on the phobic individuals' self-efficacy regarding how
over the procedures used. Additionally, the ‘‘tape recorder''
well they can execute the requisite behavior rather than on the
participants might have taken on a longer-term responsibility of
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
self-help after the official end of treatment since this version
required a higher personal responsibility from the participants.
In contrast to study,
found acrophobic thoughts in a clinical
found the therapist's role in treating
acrophobic group (N = 58) to be related to faulty thinking statements
acrophobia to be important. The authors compared the exposure
about themselves, but only when accompanied by reports of
model with the self-efficacy model in 32 participants with severe
discomfort or stress. Moreover, in comparing social anxiety with
fear of heights or driving. The self-efficacy model was significantly
acrophobia, the authors found that negative self-statements played
more effective than the exposure model regarding actual behavior,
a greater role in situation-specific (e.g. acrophobia) compared with
sense of self-efficacy, anticipatory anxiety and anxiety during
more pervasive anxiety (e.g. social phobia).
performance. However, unlike the reinforced practice model of
also found perceived danger to be an
the emphasis of the self-efficacy model is not
important feature in fear of heights. Marshall and colleagues found
upon simple exposure to the feared stimuli. The latter model
that in a sample of 50 participants (29 fearless and 21 fearful of
emphasizes the quality and quantity of information as the main
heights), many of those with acrophobia had irrational beliefs
factors in the efficacy of the treatment in addition to the use of such
about catastrophic consequences that might occur when they
information when assessing their own capacities. This and other
entered a high location. They reported that height fearful
minor treatment differences lead to further studies in which
participants were more likely to believe, and repeat to themselves,
acrophobia represented the major symptom.
that the structure will collapse, a strong wind will suddenly blow
also tested the effectiveness of
them off the building, or that they will accidentally fall from the
systematic desensitization, in a study with negative practice and
balcony of a theatre.
relaxation. Negative practice helps clients learn to control their
Conversely, in a study with a smaller number of participants
anxiety symptoms by voluntarily practicing the symptoms
(N = 15), found self-efficacy (mea-
associated with anxiety, and then by attempting to make the
sured as the participants' self-assessed ability to ascend, stand and
outcomes of this practice as close as possible to the involuntary
look down, in each of the balconies of a 10-story building) to be
anxiety response. Participants (n = 58) participated in six weekly
more accurate in predicting acrophobic behavior than perceived
treatment sessions and it was found that both negative practice
danger and anxiety arousal. In fact, four of the 15 participants gave
and desensitization produced a substantial reduction in the
very low or zero perceived danger ratings for all height tasks at pre-
test. These are nevertheless unusual findings since more recent
also compared the systematic
studies ) confirm danger expec-
desensitization and self-efficacy models in participants with
tancies related to heights as good prediction factors.
severe acrophobia. The authors again concluded that the self-
examined differences between
efficacy model was more effective than desensitization when
participants with acrophobia (n = 59) and without acrophobia
treating acrophobic participants in respect to anxious behaviors
(n = 59) regarding their danger expectancies before and during
associated with heights, with improved perceptions of self-efficacy
exposure to heights. Before exposure, participants with acrophobia
and reduced anticipatory anxiety, and danger-related thoughts.
estimated a higher probability of falling, worse consequences from
Once again, this treatment relied on the mastery of subtasks
the fall, and also considered their anxiety levels as more
including reaching intermediate goals, physical support (holding
appropriate to the situation than the control group's anxiety
the client's arm), modeling (demonstration), elimination of
levels. The estimates of falling increased when the participants
defensive maneuvers (e.g. rigidity or standing still), and various
started to climb a ladder, whereas the control group's estimates did
other performances.
not. The authors argued that insight in acrophobia was diminished
In a similar vein, performed two studies of
in the clinical group, contrary to the usual finding that individuals
participants with acrophobia. In the first study, the total duration
with phobias have considerable insight into the inappropriateness
of the exposure was ignored and the participants were exposed to
and excessiveness of their distress (e.g. ).
stimuli only until their anxiety came down to a baseline. The study
Later studies by and
also assessed the effect of the use of self-statements as a coping
appeared to contradict the
strategy. In the second study, the total exposure time remained
findings of in so far as they concluded
constant, varying only the exposure frequency, sometimes
that perceived danger and anxious arousal did not significantly
intermittently for brief periods, sometimes continuously until
predict acrophobic behavior. In fact, until its fourth edition in 1994,
the baseline was reached. The brief exposures resulted in little or
the DSM included in the diagnostic criteria the requirement that
no change, while the other procedures reduced fear. The inefficacy
the phobic individual understood fear as irrational and dispropor-
of brief exposures is consistent with other theories, such as the
tionate to the danger imposed by the object or situation. But in
incubation theory of which suggested that brief
1998, proposed a sub-category of
exposures may even cause an increase of fear. In this respect,
poor insight similar to the one used to specify obsessive
excessive brief exposure can have similar effects to those of
compulsive disorders. This suggestion was made after their data
avoidance, thereby perpetuating the fear.
indicated that some people actually have poor insight as to the real
All of these studies were important steps in the process of
danger of the feared situations, even when distant from them. In
validating different treatment programs for acrophobia. Never-
their study, 64 participants (34 women and 30 men) with
theless, the studies were essentially designed to explore and
acrophobia were placed on the same ladder of 27 steps used in
compare acrophobia treatment models, and were not specifically
an earlier study Participants were then
designed to understand the trigger features associated with fear of
asked to predict their probability of falling. On the basis of this, two
heights, as the visual-cliff studies had done with human infants
groups were created: a poor insight group composed of those
and animals.
predicting more than a 50% chance of falling, and a good insightgroup composed of those predicting less than 50% chance of falling.
5. Acrophobia therapy efficacy: the role of cognitive processes
Interestingly, each group's average estimate for the probability offalling was very different. On the last step, the good insight
Other early treatment studies of acrophobia focused on the
acrophobic group estimated a 9.84% chance of falling and the poor
cognitive processes that occur during exposure to heights (
insight group estimated a probability of 88.6%.
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
These findings suggest that it is possible to develop a fear of
entertainment industries (for review see
heights via a number of distinct pathways. Some participants have
; When applied to the health
fear despite being sure that they will not fall, and others feel a real
sciences, the military origins of VR mean it is perhaps most apposite
danger from this situation. This would also explain why some
for the treatment of the fear of flying (aerophobia) and the fear of
researchers find danger expectations to be important as predictors
heights (acrophobia). In fact, during testing of a VR device in 1992,
of fear () and others do not
found that it produced emotional and
One feature universal to all studies is the presence
physical symptoms in the participant that seemed to be phobic
of irrationality about the specific height situation. This might be a
behaviors, similar to those of a person reacting to a feared situation.
signal that a trigger, exclusively related to height environments, is
The device was a ‘‘flying carpet'' that flew in a particular direction
causing some people to ‘overreact'. Overall, these studies suggest
depending on which area was stepped upon. The observers then
the absence of an underlying ‘‘general anxiousness'' in participants
realized that the symptoms were not due to motion sickness but to
with acrophobia. Instead, there appears to be a specific cata-
real fear (see The first clinical VR application for
strophic cognitively based association with height situations, in
acrophobia was described by while
particular during exposure to height stimuli.
approached the same question
used VR for aerophobia.
by combining self-report with a physiological measure (heart rate).
Currently, VRET is seen as a viable alternative to exposure in
Their study group was comprised of 19 participants (nine men and
vivo since it can elicit fear and anxiety ) thereby
10 women) for whom acrophobia imposed marked and distressing
functioning as an alternative mode of inducing exposure (
impairment on their everyday life. Since the study was designed to
Additionally, when using a VR
assess the influence of cognitive processes, half of the participants
system, the therapist and patient do not need to leave the
were instructed in adaptive thinking during an exposure in vivo
consulting room. This implies a saving of time and money (e.g.
task and the other half underwent exposure alone. In contrast to
the studies described above, the authors concluded that cognition
but also brings the
was unimportant in the process of habituation to heights, since
benefit of not risking possible public embarrassment for the
changes in participant anxiety during treatment did not precede
patient as well as preserving their confidentiality
changes in heart rate (HR). The authors also found that although
). VR can, in this way, be
only the group instructed in adaptive thinking changed in the
an intermediate step towards live exposure, i.e. a step between a
cognitive measures, participants improved significantly on HR and
completely protected consulting room and the ‘threatening real
the behavioral measure in both conditions.
world'. However, VR may also represent an end in itself since it
The role of cognitive factors in contributing to the etiology and
offers the ability to render height-related situations that are not
maintenance of acrophobia remains unresolved. Many of the more
easily accessible to the therapist and patient, e.g. an airplane, a
recent attempts to understand their role have made use of virtual
high-rise building, etc. ().
reality (VR) technology. Exposed to modern, high-fidelity systems,
VRET may also be useful as a form of therapy for clients who
participants can become thoroughly immersed in the task, in
have difficulty with imaginal exposure The
objects, entities and event perception (
VR-based treatment can also be more precisely tailored to each
This so-called ‘presence' in the virtual environment (see
client, offering greater control over stimuli and even the possibility
then becomes a tool for
of creating stimuli of greater magnitude than one could experience
exploring insight in people with acrophobia. Knowing that an
in the real world Equally, by creating stimuli of
augmented sense of presence is related to an increased amount of
lesser magnitude than is experienced in the real world, partici-
anxiety in individuals with a specific phobia (
pants may start treatment even if they are too anxious and fearful
), it is possible to evaluate thoughts of acrophobics with poor
to be treated via real exposure
insight, in a realistic VR environment. Here participants would
have a similar visuo-vestibular input to a real situation, but
The flexibility and confidentiality offered by VR brings with it
without any physical danger. Conversely, acrophobics with good
another important advantage in that it can encourage more people
insight might be susceptible to less cognitive bias and hence more
to seek treatment. Indeed, one assessment of students with a fear
prone to a visuo-vestibular susceptibility. Given its potential to
of spiders showed that almost 90% would prefer VR exposure over
provide these answers, this review will now evaluate what has
in vivo exposure therapy
been done in the area of VR and acrophobia research.
This is particularly important for participants with phobiassince they do not seek help readily (According to
6. The potential utility of VR in treating acrophobia
, help-seeking is higher among individuals withagoraphobia (fear of open spaces) and social phobia, than for other
Since 1995, there has been a significant lack of studies on the
specific phobias.
treatment of acrophobia outside the field of virtual environments.
The combined benefits of VR approaches suggest that it holds
This seems primarily due to the advent of virtual reality exposure
great promise as a therapeutic tool for enhancing acrophobia
therapy (VRET) in 1996, to which this review now turns. Although VR
treatment outcomes. Since the technology in this area is
is only a recent field of technological application in research and
developing rapidly, there is good reason to believe that this same
clinical practice, many of its core concepts have been explored and
technology will soon be economically accessible to private
developed over the past 50 years. Its roots go back to the
therapists. Currently, there is ongoing VR work on posttraumatic
development of the flight simulator in the American aerospace
stress disorder, eating disorders, sexual dysfunction, schizophrenic
and defense industries during the Second World War
hallucinations, and addictions (for review see
). From the Army's first digital computer (the ENIAC) in the
1940s, to the Air Force's research into visualization helmets (VCASS)in the 1980s, the American armed forces have always been the main
7. Specific acrophobia treatments in virtual environments
source of funding for the most important innovations in computertechnology. Besides the military, advances in computer graphics
According to and
technology in general have also been driven by the film and
can be credited with the first use of VR in the treatment of
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
the acrophobia, although one might better describe it as an altered,
A subsequent study by took a more
rather than virtual, reality. In that work, Schneider used binoculars
structured approach than any previous studies. The authors began
with inverted lenses to alter the perception of depth, so as to
by reproducing the real-world exposure site in VR. The authors
magnify the sensation of height during a process of exposure in a
then applied three 1-h sessions to 33 acrophobic participants (16
real context. This procedure was published as a case study. The
exposures in vivo and 17 with VR) and noted that the VR treatment
participant, a 40-year-old man who lived in a city filled with
was as effective as exposure in vivo in combating anxiety and
skyscrapers, looked out from an eighth floor with the binoculars.
avoidance. The measures used were the Acrophobia Questionnaire
The effect of the binoculars was to give the impression of standing
(AQ) (the Attitude Towards Heights Questionnaire
on the 56th floor, and the removal of the binoculars at the end of
(ATHQ) ) and a Behavioral Avoidance Test
each viewing trial produced an immediate and dramatic decrease
(BAT). This method, in contrast to the previous studies, showed
in apparent height, thus helping to overcome the participant's
improvements not only in the self-report but also in the BAT. The
results remained the same in a six month follow-up with 29 of the
By the early to mid-1990s, computer-based VR systems became
participants. The authors claimed that: ‘‘Given the chronic nature
widely available and clinicians began to consider their use in
of the acrophobia (mean duration of 31.5 years) in our patients, it is
treatments. In the first reported study, exposed 30
highly unlikely that the results are due to spontaneous recovery''
acrophobic participants to simulated height situations. After one
week of treatment, 90% of the participants were capable of coping
Taken as a whole, there is now a considerable body of evidence
with real situations. A follow-up at 30 months revealed that 27 of
that VR exposure is an effective means of treating acrophobia
the participants were quite capable of using a glass elevator whilst
looking outside. This study seemed to indicate that the treatment
was beneficial in both the short and long term. Unfortunately, this
study lacked any kind of rigorous analysis and it is possibly the
encouraging fact, more recent studies have moved on to use VR to
reason why a study by is
help explore acrophobic triggers and features of treatment. For
more often cited as the first clinical application of a VR system to
acrophobic disorders. In their paper they presented a single case
tested 26 participants with acrophobia in VRET sessions and
study involving the assessment and treatment of a 19-year-old
coping cognitive self-statements. Results showed improvements
regardless of the addition of coping self-statements. Despite this,
The success of this study motivated the authors to continue
the importance of cognitive factors surfaced in a study by
their research, and in the same year they published a more
who found that D-Cycloserine (DCS) (a cognitive
elaborate study in which they assessed 478 university students
enhancer) combined with exposure therapy in a VR environment
and selected 20 that had presented with a substantial fear and
resulted in significantly larger reductions in acrophobia symptoms
avoidance of heights (). Of these 20, 12 were
compared with placebo and exposure therapy. The treatment was
selected to participate in VR therapy, while the remaining eight
conducted in VR and 27 participants (11 men and 16 women) with
acted as an untreated control group. The treatment consisted of
acrophobia were assigned a placebo plus VRET (n = 10); 50 mg of
seven weekly sessions of 35–45 min. Anxiety, avoidance and stress
DCS plus VRET (n = 8), or 500 mg of DCS and VRET (n = 9). The
were significantly diminished among the 12 students involved in
difference between the 50 mg and 500 mg was not found to be
VR therapy but not in the control group. Although a clear advance
statistically significant. DCS and VRET showed maintained
in the assessment of VR therapy, the study was still open to
improvements at three months follow up reevaluation in 21 of
criticism as it lacked a control group treated with real exposure (i.e.
the 27 participants.
in vivo), provided no follow-up tests, and included no formalassessment of phobic avoidance.
8. Examining the triggers of acrophobic behavior using VR
A year later, reported a case study in
which they employed eight VR sessions lasting between 15 and
8.1. Visuo-vestibular triggers
28 min each. The treatment was conducted using standardprotocols, beginning with the least threatening situation, as in
Some of the earliest attempts to understand fear of heights
the classic treatment of systematic desensitization (
independently of cognitive factors were made by
and reinforced practice ). The Subjective Units of
Discomfort (SUDS) questionnaire (was administered
postulated a physiological mechanism for heights vertigo, separate
every 2–5 min. One month after treatment, the results indicated
from psychological factors. Similar to motion sickness, heights
significant improvement, with a decrease in both anxiety
vertigo was, in their opinion, due to a conflict between vision and
symptoms and the avoidance of anxiety-producing situations.
the somatosensory and vestibular systems. They postulated that
A few years later, directly compared
such a discrepancy occurs when the vestibular and somatosensory
the effectiveness of VR with a real-world exposure control. In
receptors sense a body shift (the natural lateral head sway – 2 cm)
comparison to the previous studies by
which is not detected by the visual system, due to the large
, the hardware used was less sophisticated, offering
distance to the nearest viewed objects. ‘‘This conflict might be
just 10 frames per second to define movement through the visual
resolved by increasing the postural sway and thereby reactivating
environment. All the participants received VR treatment in the first
visual control'' p. 515). In other words, they
two sessions and then kept receiving treatment in a real-world
proposed that one reason for the increase in sway might be that
environment. They discovered that the subsequent real exposure
participants are attempting exploratory behavior aimed at
did not lead to a significant improvement in the measurements of
increasing visual balance feedback (
avoidance in the Acrophobia Questionnaire ) and of
This make intuitive sense because the further a
Attitudes Towards Heights ). Unfortu-
viewer is from a visual target (e.g. height above a ground plane) the
nately, all participants were exposed to the VR environment first
smaller the motion parallax cue (provided by movement of the
and this proved to be so effective that little or no room was left for
head) becomes. This can be compensated by exaggerating head
the later real-world treatment to produce measurable improve-
movements and body sway. As point out; an
alternative explanation for increased body sway is that it is simply
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
an indicator of poor control in the absence of strong visual cues,
small sway area, despite a large sway locus length. Acrophobic
rather than an attempt to drive the visual system more strongly.
participants' legs were observed to be shivering'' (
A growing body of literature shows that this conflict between
p. 502). In a second group (n = 12) with two acrophobics,
vestibular and visual information can lead not only to motion
participants were blindfolded and guided to a roof without being
sickness, but to anxiety as well
told where they were going. In this condition, nonacrophobic
participants lessened their body sway when the blindfold was
A recent study from showed
removed. In contrast, acrophobic participants' sway increased.
that fear of heights was probably not due to any vestibular
These results seem to contradict those of
malfunctioning, but rather possible cognitive bias in interpreting
who found that fear of falling instigates a stiffened
bodily sensations, thereby partially supporting
posture in participants.
conclusions. Thus, it is possible that individuals
There is a growing literature studying the link between anxiety
with acrophobia tend to interpret visuo-vestibular conflict as an
about falling and postural control (e.g.
alerting sign of a possible fall.
Recently, corroborated the non-associative
mode of fear acquisition in acrophobia by reporting evidence for
These studies originally
the existence of a physiological mechanism responsible of
focused on the idea that a fear of falling in older adults was
triggering the phobic behavior. Using a dual-task paradigm, they
negatively impacting on their posture. To investigate this, the
found lower scores in balance stability (on a mobile platform) in
researchers studied individuals' standing posture at different
participants with fear of heights. The authors also noted that the
surface heights above the ground. It was hypothesized that the
addition of a manual tracking task significantly impaired postural
threat or risk of injury associated with a loss of balance would
stability in acrophobics. The task consisted involved moving a
increase when standing at a high surface height. This situation was
joystick to keep a computer displayed circle inside a square. It was
supposed to be similar to the one experienced by an individual who
also found that individuals with acrophobia had lower scores than
has a fear of falling. Even though they were not specifically
controls in the manual tracking task even when the platform
interested in acrophobia, the fact that these researchers system-
remained stationary, suggesting abnormalities in the visual
atically varied height in their studies makes their findings of
perception of movement ).
relevance to the study of acrophobia as well.
In their review of VR technology as a research tool,
What these authors generally reported was that the central
describe how VR might be used to
nervous system imposes modifications to postural control when
investigate the role of vision in acrophobia. In particular the asked:
fall anxiety increases (; ),
‘‘does pure visual information elicit acrophobia, or must appro-
altering posture by reducing the amplitude and increasing the
priate head orientation, signaled by neck proprioception and
frequency of postural sway (
vestibular cues (e.g. looking down from a high bridge) accompany
This is achieved by co-contraction of ankle joint agonist/
the visual information? To answer this, one can decouple the
antagonist muscle pairs, resulting in a tighter regulation of
participant's visual stimulation from his or her head and eye
centre of mass (COM) This accommoda-
movements, so that exactly the same visual information is
tion may serve to reduce the permitted range of COM
presented with the head facing downward (normal coupling) or
displacement and minimize the probability of a loss of balance
facing upward (decoupled)'' (, p. 561). These
(). The increased frequency and decreased
postural behaviors and visuo-vestibular cues can best be
amplitude in elevated environments are usually accompanied by
manipulated and measured in VR environments since they are
an adoption of more posterior COM and centre of pressure
extremely difficult to control in the real world (
position (COP) compared to the non-elevated environments
Research along these lines will also allow the exploration of the
These results are important because they show the possibility
nature of fear of flying and its similarities and differences with fear
that individuals with acrophobia have an extremely stiffened
of heights. The two specific phobias are clustered together
posture that impoverishes performance in a similar mechanism to
suggesting that the situational and the natural environment
that of Parkinson's Disease (
phobia type could share a common underlying theme
In patients with Parkinson's disease, falls were
Nonetheless it is also conceivable that fear of flying is
found to be correlated with rigidity and not with tremor. In the
composed of one or more other fears, being an expression of
acrophobia case, a similar phenomenon might occur, but due to a
several subtypes of fear and phobias such as fear of accidents, need
postural strategy problem. It is plausible that height fearful
of control, claustrophobia, acrophobia, social phobia, and symp-
individuals' strategies to control balance become critically
toms of panic attacks (
impaired and stiffened in the absence of visual cues or in the
presence of unexpected visual environments. These studies on
VR will assist in answering questions regarding the influence of the
postural control reveal the need to further explore the influence of
vestibular system in postural stability control and acrophobia.
postural threat on the control of upright stance.
8.2. Postural triggers
8.3. Visual dependence and motion
found similar results
It is well known that motor action and depth vision are linked
to in which normal individuals exposed to
() and that some participants rely more
heights exhibited increased body sway. In a study with 30
on visual cues to control balance than others (
participants in which eight had acrophobia,
Participants with an increased dependence
noticed that the frequency and amplitude of
on visual field information (FD) are characterized as less physically
sway increased significantly (measured with a stabilometer) at
stable and more reliant on visual cues (particularly motion visual
10.2 m (on a roof) but showed no change at 1 or 2 m. In participants
cues) for controlling body stabilization. By contrast, the partici-
with acrophobia (n = 6, since two were unable to stand on the roof)
pants not dependent on this information (FI) are more physically
the postural pattern was of: ‘‘small and frequent vibrations with a
stable and rely less on vestibular and somatosensory cues (
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
Moreover, FD participants
(Much of the subsequent research has
have a greater sensitivity to motion cues due to self-movements
been focused on exploring the viability of this technology in
than FI participants
treatment. It is now known that both the real-world and VRET
This increased reliance on visual cues may be a vulnerability
regimens are highly effective for treating acrophobia. A recent
factor that promotes disequilibrium during certain situations of
meta-analysis ) presented estimates
daily living that include complex environments, moving visual
of effect size for VR treatments across a range of phobias and
fields and heights (and thus may predispose
anxiety disorder in comparison to in vivo exposure and control
participants to the development of agoraphobia or acrophobia
conditions and found VRET was ‘‘slightly, but significantly, more
effective than exposure in vivo, the gold standard in the field''
It is possible that self-motion has an important role in triggering
(p. 568). Because VR hardware is
fear of heights. As previously noted (i.e.,
becoming more reasonably priced
), acrophobic participants experience high
and VR systems can create a greater variety of stimuli and
levels of anxiety when required to move laterally at a fixed height.
situations applicable to treatment, practitioners and researchers
further suggested that oscillation phases of
are still actively exploring the potential of this technology (for
locomotion, involving monopedal stance (on one leg), could
review see ). Overall,
increase balance instability especially in field-dependent partici-
VR offers some practical advantages over real-world exposure such
pants. Visual dependence seems to increase with height, and
as (1) better control of the situation by the therapist; (2) avoidance
participants can be especially visually dependent when walking on
of public embarrassment; (3) preservation of confidentiality; (4)
a narrow support Although speculative at this
maintenance of the protective environment of the therapist's office
stage, it is possible that perceptual estimates of heights relate to
(see also ).
cognitive and other fear-relevant biases when they are activated by
Besides the utility of VR as a tool for treatment, it also offers a
concerns about acting in the environment, as recently noticed by
route into further investigating and understanding acrophobia.
Our own studies based on VR treatment suggest to us that motion
researchers found that high fear of height participants (n = 35)
combined with simulated height, rather than height per se, can
estimated the vertical extent of a two-story balcony ledge to be
trigger phobic responses (
higher compared to 36 participants with low acrophobic symp-
In fact, between 1993 and 2001, the majority of
toms. Additionally, given that perception informs us about the
studies were related to the etiology of acrophobia and suggested a
opportunities for action and the costs associated with these actions
non-associative account. Nonetheless, both the non-associative
proposed that geographical
and the behaviorist account accept the important role of learning
height perception is influenced by the risks of the inherent dangers
to overcome the fear, whether it is learned or innate. The relative
of behaving near the edge of a high drop-off. This suggests a
magnitude of each vulnerability and learning factor in the
possible interactive model in which cognitive factors (e.g. fear of
diathesis remains a topic of current debate. At the moment, we
falling; catastrophic interpretations), perceptual factors (e.g. visual
would tend to agree with
dependence), learning factors (e.g. prior exposure to heights), and
suggestion to explore the possible mechanisms of diathesis-
biological factors (e.g. heredity) can interact, provoking either
stress interactions in the development of anxiety and fear. In our
habituation or extreme fear in height-related environments.
particular case, this review suggests that the exploration of visuo-vestibular and motion mechanisms that might be present in
9. Summary and conclusion
acrophobia is also important. It is clear from seminal studies inchildren (e.g. Campos, 1978; ;
It emerges from the present review that numerous studies with
) that a fear of heights accompanies the emergence of
acrophobic participants aimed to investigate the efficacies of
self-locomotion (crawling) and that habituation processes can aid
different treatment models and not necessarily to study acro-
in overcoming a fear of heights, supporting the idea that the
phobia itself. Between 1973 and 1992, research on participants
etiology of acrophobia is intimately linked to body movement
with acrophobia was essentially focused on comparisons between
the exposure model and the self-efficacy model. Treatment within
It seems plausible that an increased dependence on dynamic
the self-efficacy model was usually more structured and had more
visual cues for controlling body stabilization might be a key factor
therapeutic components, such as the mastery of subtasks,
in the predispositions to the development of acrophobia (
physical support, modeling and elimination of defensive man-
euvers and this in turn lead to it producing the most effective
between motion and fear still requires systematic examination. In
clinical approach for the treatment of acrophobia. Although
the coming years research investigating the links between
perhaps not the main focus of their work, the self-efficacy
acrophobia, motion triggers, and implications for acrophobia
approach also provided important insights into the root causes
treatment, will draw heavily on VR technology, which is proving
and triggers of acrophobia. For example, noticed
to be an invaluable tool for measuring physiological and
that a person's judgment about his or her ability and confidence to
behavioral indices in response to controlled visual and motion
perform various specific actions (self-efficacy) is a major
triggers. VR is probably the most useful tool to decouple visual
determinant of human behavior. In a similar vein, we and other
information, head orientation, proprioception and vestibular
researchers have begun to recognize the crucial role of movement
cues, and independently evaluate their role in acrophobia. As
in generating acrophobic responses in sufferers, making it a key
such, we look forward to VR ushering in a period of discovery that
feature to address in treatment (
will greatly enhance our understanding of this debilitating and
). Movement has also
widespread phobia.
emerged as an important fear trigger. As we have argued in thisreview, it seems likely that motion parallax and visuo-vestibular
conflict play an important role in the development of acrophobiain some sufferers (
This work was funded by a grant from the Portuguese Science
From its outset in 1995 as a treatment of acrophobia, VR has
and Technology Foundation (ref. SFRH/BPD/26922/2006), awarded
come to dominate the treatment of numerous anxiety disorders
to the first author.
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
T. Anguera, B. K. Wiederhold, & F. Mantovani (Eds.), From communication topresence. Cognition, emotions and culture towards the ultimate communicative
Abelson, J. L., & Curtis, G. C. (1989). Cardiac and neuroendocrine responses to exposure
experience. Festschrift in honor of Luigi Anolli (pp. 25–45). Amsterdam: IOS Press.
therapy in height phobics: desynchrony within the ‘‘physiological response sys-
Cohen, D. C. (1977). Comparison of self-report and overt behavioral procedures for
tem''. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 27(5), 561–567.
assessing acrophobia. Behavior Therapy, 8(1), 17–23.
Adkin, A. L., Frank, J. S., Carpenter, M. G., & Peysar, G. W. (2000). Postural control is
Craske, M. G., & Waters, A. M. (2005). Panic disorder, phobias, and generalized anxiety
scaled to level of postural threat. Gait & Posture, 12(2), 87–93.
disorder. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 197–225.
Adkin, A. L., Frank, J. S., Carpenter, M. G., & Peysar, G. W. (2002). Fear of falling modifies
Davey, G. C. L., Menzies, R., & Gallardo, B. (1997). Height phobia and biases in the
anticipatory postural control. Experimental Brain Research, 143(2), 160–170.
interpretation of bodily sensations: some links between acrophobia and agora-
Adolph, K. E. (2002). Babies' steps make giant strides toward a science of development.
phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(11), 997–1001.
Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 86–90.
Doogan, S., & Thomas, G. V. (1992). Origins of fear of dogs in adults and children—the
Adolph, K. E. (2008). Learning to move. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3),
role of conditioning processes and prior familiarity with dogs. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 30(4), 387–394.
Agras, W. S., & Jacob, R. G. (1981). Phobia: nature and measurement. In: M. Mavissa-
Emmelkamp, P. G. M. (1982). Phobic and obsessive-compulsive disorders: theory,
kalian & D. H. Barlow (Eds.), Phobia. Psychological and Pharmacological Treatment
research, and practice. New York: Plenum.
(pp. 35–62). New York: The Guilford Press.
Emmelkamp, P. G. M., Bruynzeel, M., Drost, L., & van der Mast, C. A. P. G. (2001). Virtual
Alpers, G. W., & Adolph, D. (2008). Exposure to heights in a theme park: fear, dizziness,
reality treatment in acrophobia: a comparison with exposure in vivo. Cyberpsy-
and body sway. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 591–601.
chology & Behavior, 4(3), 335–339.
Antony, M., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Heterogeneity among specific phobia
Emmelkamp, P. G. M., & Felten, M. (1985). The process of exposure in vivo: cognitive
types in DSM-IV. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 1089–1100.
and physiological changes during treatment of acrophobia. Behaviour Research and
APA. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington
Therapy, 23(2), 219–223.
DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Emmelkamp, P. G. M., Krijn, M., Hulsbosch, L., de Vries, S., Schuemie, M. J., & van der
APA. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington,
Mast, C. A. P. G. (2002). Virtual reality treatment versus exposure in vivo: a
DC: American Psychiatric Association.
comparative evaluation in acrophobia. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 40(5), 509–
Arntz, A., Rauner, M., & van den Hout, M. (1995). If I feel anxious, there must be
danger'': ex-consequentia reasoning in inferring danger in anxiety disorders.
Essau, C. A., Conradt, J., & Petermann, F. (2000). Frequency, comorbility, and psycho-
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(8), 917–925.
social impairment of specific phobia in adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child
Baker, B. L., Cohen, D. C., & Saunders, J. T. (1973). Self-directed desensitization for
Psychology, 29(2), 221–231.
acrophobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 11(1), 79–89.
Eysenck, H. J. (1968). A theory of the incubation of anxiety/fear responses. Behaviour
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy—toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
Research and Therapy, 6, 309–321.
Psychological Review, 84(2), 191–215.
Field, A. P., Argyris, N. G., & Knowles, K. A. (2001). Who's afraid of the big bad wolf: a
Bles, W., Kapteyn, T. S., Brandt, T., & Arnold, F. (1980). The mechanism of physiological
prospective paradigm to test Rachman's indirect pathways in children. Behaviour
height vertigo. II. Posturography. Acta oto-laryngologica, 89, 534–540.
Research and therapy, 39, 1259–1276.
Bond, N., & Siddle, D. A. (1996). The preparedness account of social phobia: some data
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: exposure to corrective
and alternative explanations. In: R. M. Rapee (Ed.), Current Controversies in the
information. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1), 20–35.
Anxiety Disorders (pp. 291–316). London: Guilford Press.
Forsyth, J. P., & Chorpita, B. F. (1997). Unearthing the nonassociative origins of fears and
Boffino, C. C., de Sa´, C. S., Gorenstein, C., Brown, R. G., Basile, L. F., & Ramos, R. T. (2008).
phobias: a rejoinder. Journal of Behaviour Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
Fear of heights: cognitive performance and postural control. European Archives of
28(4), 297–305.
Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience (pub ahead of print).
Fredrikson, M., Annas, P., Fischer, H., & Wik, G. (1996). Gender and age differences in the
Botella, C., Banos, R. M., Perpina, C., Villa, H., Alcaniz, M., & Rey, M. (1998). Virtual reality
prevalence of specific fears and phobias. Behaviour. Research and Therapy, 34(1),
treatment of claustrophobia: a case report. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36,
Garcia, P. A., Hoffman, H. G., See, S. K., Tsai, A., & Botella, C. (2001). Redefining
Boyd, J. H., Rae, D. S., Thompson, J. W., Burns, B. J., Bourdon, K., Locke, B. Z., et al. (1990).
therapeutic success with virtual reality exposure therapy. CyberPsychology and
Phobia: prevalence and risk factors. Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology,
Behaviour, 4(3), 341–348.
25, 314–323.
Gatev, P., Thomas, S., Kepple, T., & Hallett, M. (1999). Feedforward ankle strategy of
Brandt, T., Arnold, F., Bles, W., & Kapteyn, T. S. (1980). The mechanism of physiological
balance during quiet stance in adults. Journal of Physiology-London, 514(3), 915–928.
height vertigo. I. Theoretical approach and psychophysics. Acta oto-laryngologica,
Gibson, E. J., & Walk, R. D. (1960). The visual cliff. Scientific American, 202, 64–71.
89, 513–523.
Glantz, K., Durlach, N. I., Barnet, R. C., & Aviles, W. A. (1996). Virtual reality (VR) for
Brown, L. A., & Frank, J. S. (1997). Postural compensations to the potential consequences
psychotherapy: from the physical to the social environment. Psychotherapy, 33(3),
of instability: kinematics. Gait & Posture, 6(2), 89–97.
Brown, L. A., Polych, M. A., & Doan, J. B. (2006). The effect of anxiety on the regulation of
Graham, J., & Gaffan, E. A. (1997). Fear of water in children and adults: etiology and
upright standing among younger and older adults. Gait & Posture, 24(4), 397–405.
familial effects. Behaviour Research and therapy, 35(2), 91–108.
Burns, L. E. (1980). The epidemiology of fears and phobias in general practice. Journal of
Grau, O. (2004). Virtual art. From illusion to immersion. Boston: MIT Press.
International Medical Research, 8(3), 1–7.
Hein, A. (1972). Acquiring components of visually guided behavior. In: Pick, A. (Ed.).
Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hernstein, M. J., &
Minnesota symposia on child psychology. Vol. 6 (pp.53–68).Minneapolis: University
Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1(2), 149–219.
of Minnesota Press.
Campos, J. J., Bertenthal, B. I., & Kermoian, R. (1992). Early experience and emotional
Held, R., & Hein, A. (1963). Movement-produced stimulation in the development of
development: the emergence of wariness of heights. Psychological Science, 3(1), 61–
visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56,
Campos, J. J., Hiatt, S., Ramsay, D., Henderson, C., & Svejda, M. (1978). The emergence of
Hodges, L. F., Kooper, R., Meyer, T. C., Rothbaum, B. O., Opdyke, D., Degraaff, J. J., et al.
fear on the visual cliff. In: The development of affect. New York: Plenum Press.
(1995a). Virtual environments for treating the fear of heights. Computer, 28(7), 27–
Carpenter, M. G., Frank, J. S., & Silcher, C. P. (1999). Surface height effects on postural
control: a hypothesis for a stiffness strategy for stance. Journal of Vestibular
Hodges, L. F., Kooper, R., Meyer, T. C., Rothbaum, B. O., Opdyke, D., Graaf, J. J., et al.
Research-Equilibrium & Orientation, 9(4), 277–286.
(1995b). Virtual environments for treating the fear of heights. Computer. Innovative
Carpenter, M. G., Frank, J. S., Silcher, C. P., & Peysar, G. W. (2001). The influence of
Technology for Computer Professionals, 28, 27–34.
postural threat on the control of upright stance. Experimental Brain Research,
Howard, W. A., Murphy, S. M., & Clarke, J. C. (1983). The nature and treatment of fear of
138(2), 210–218.
flying: a controlled investigation. Behavior Therapy, 14, 557–567.
Cavanagh, K., & Shapiro, D. A. (2004). Computer treatment for common mental health
Ijsselsteijn, W. A. (2003). Presence in the past: what can we learn from media history?
problems. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60, 239–251.
In: G. Riva, F. Davide, & W. A. Ijsselsteijn (Eds.), Being there: concepts, effects and
Chapman, T. F. (1997). The Epidemiology of fears and phobias. In: G. L. C. Davey (Ed.),
measurement of user presence in synthetic environments (pp. 17–40). Amsterdam:
Phobias. A handbook of theory, research and treatment (pp. 129–138). Chichester:
Isableu, B., Ohlmann, T., Cremieux, J., & Amblard, B. (1998). How dynamic visual field
Choi, Y. H., Jang, D. P., Ku, J. H., Shin, M. B., & Kim, S. I. (2001). Short-term treatment of
dependence-independence interacts with the visual contribution to postural
acrophobia with virtual reality therapy (VRT): a case report. Cyberpsychology &
control. Human Movement Science, 17(3), 367–391.
Behavior, 4(3), 349–354.
Isableu, B., Ohlmann, T., Cremieux, J., & Amblard, B. (2003). Differential approach to
Coelho, C. M., Santos, J. A., Silva, C. F., Tichoon, J., Hine, T. J., & Wallis, G. (2008a). The role
strategies of segmental stabilisation in postural control. Experimental Brain
of self-motion in acrophobia treatment. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 11(6), 723–
Research, 150(2), 208–221.
Jacob, R. G., Redfern, M. S., & Furman, J. M. (1995). Optic flow-induced sway in anxiety
Coelho, C. M., Santos, J. A., Silverio, J., & Silva, C. F. (2006a). Virtual reality and
disorders associated with space and motion discomfort. Journal of Anxiety Dis-
acrophobia: one-year follow-up and case study. Cyberpsychology & Behavior,
orders, 9, 411–425.
9(3), 336–341.
Jacob, R. G., Woody, S. R., Clark, D. B., Lilienfeld, S. O., Hirsch, B. E., Kucera, G. D., et al.
Coelho, C. M., Silva, C. F., Santos, J. A., Tichon, J., & Wallis, G. (2008b). Contrasting the
(1993). Discomfort with space and motion: a possible marker of vestibular
effectiveness and efficiency of virtual reality and real environments in the treat-
dysfunction assessed by the situational characteristics questionnaire. Journal of
ment of acrophobia. Psychology Journal, 6(2), 203–216.
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 15, 299–324.
Coelho, C. M., Tichon, J., Hine, T. J., Wallis, G., & Riva, G. (2006b). Media presence and
Joh, A. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2006). Learning from falling. Child Development, 77(1),
inner presence: the sense of presence in virtual reality technologies. In: G. Riva, M.
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
Kendler, K. S., Myers, J., & Prescott, C. A. (2002). The etiology of phobias—an evaluation
North, M. M., & North, S. M. (1996). Virtual psychotherapy. Journal of Medicine and
of the stress-diathesis model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(3), 242–248.
Virtual Reality, 1, 28–32.
Kitamura, F., & Matsunaga, K. (1990). Field-dependence and body balance. Perceptual
North, M. M., North, S. M., & Coble, J. R. (1996). Virtual reality therapy an innovative
and Motor Skills, 71(3), 723–734.
paradigm. Colorado Springs: IPI Press.
Koller, W. C., Glatt, S., Vetereoverfield, B., & Hassanein, R. (1989). Falls and Parkinsons-
O'brien, G. T. (1981). Clinical treatment of specific phobias. In: M. Mavissakalian & D. H.
disease. Clinical Neuropharmacology, 12(2), 98–105.
Barlow (Eds.), Phobia. Psychological and pharmacological treatment (pp. 63–102).
Krijn, M., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., Biemond, R., de Wilde de Ligny, C., Schuemie, M. J., &
New York: The Guilford Press.
van der Mast, C. A. P. G. (2004a). Treatment of acrophobia in virtual reality: the role
Pendleton, M. G., & Higgins, R. L. (1983). A comparison of negative practice and
of immersion and presence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(2), 229–239.
systematic desensitization in the treatment of acrophobia. Journal of Behavior
Krijn, M., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., Olafsson, R. P., & Biemond, R. (2004b). Virtual reality
Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 14, 317–323.
exposure therapy of anxiety disorders: a review. Clinical Psychology Review, 24(3),
Pimentel, K., & Teixeira, K. (1993). Virtual reality. Through the new looking glass. New
Krijn, M., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., O´lafsson, R. P., Shuemie, M. J., & van der Mast, C. A. P. G.
Poulton, R., Davies, S., Menzies, R. G., Langley, J. D., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Evidence for a
(2007). Do self-statements enhance the effectiveness of virtual reality exposure
non-associative model of the acquisition of a fear of heights. Behaviour Research
therapy? A comparative evaluation in acrophobia. CyberPsychology and Behavior,
and Therapy, 36(5), 537–544.
10, 362–370.
Poulton, R., & Menzies, R. G. (2002). Non-associative fear acquisition: a review of the
Lamson, R. (1994). Virtual therapy of anxiety disorders. CyberEdge Journal, 4, 6–8.
evidence from retrospective and longitudinal research. Behaviour Research and
Leitenberg, H. (1976). Behavioral approaches to treatment of neuroses. In: H. Leiten-
Therapy, 40, 127–149.
berg (Ed.), Handbook of behavior modification and behavior therapy. Englewood
Poulton, R., Waldie, K. E., Menzies, R. G., Craske, M. G., & Silva, P. A. (2001). Failure to
Cliffs NJ: Prentice-Hall.
overcome ‘innate' fear: a developmental test of the non-associative model of fear
Leitenberg, H., Agras, S., Edwards, J. A., Thomson, L. E., & Wincze, J. P. (1970). Practice as
acquisition. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39(1), 29–43.
a psychotherapeutic variable: an experimental analysis within single cases. Journal
Powers, M. B., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (2008). Virtual reality exposure therapy for
of Psychiatric Research, 7, 215–225.
anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 561–569.
Leitenberg, H., Agras, W. S., Allen, R., Bitz, R., & Edwards, J. (1975). Feedback and
Price, M., & Anderson, P. (2007). The role of presence in virtual reality exposure therapy.
therapist praise during treatment of phobia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21(5), 742–751.
Psychology, 43, 396–404.
Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on
Littman, M. K. (1996). Alternative meanings through the world of virtual reality. In: K.
Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.
Vandergrift (Ed.), Mosaics of meaning: enhancing the intellectual life of young adults
Rachman, S. (2002). Fears born and bred: non-associative fear acquisition? Behaviour
through story (pp. 425–455). Lanham: Scarecrow Press.
Research and therapy, 40, 121–126.
Loftus, E. F. (2004). Memories of things unseen. Current Directions in Psychological
Reason, J. T. (1978). Motion sickness adaptation—neural mismatch model. Journal of the
Science, 13, 145–147.
Royal Society of Medicine, 71(11), 819–829.
Lombard, M., & Ditton, T. (1997). At the heart of it all: the concept of presence. Journal of
Redfern, M. S., Yardley, L., & Bronstein, A. M. (2001). Visual influences on balance.
Computer-Mediated Communication, 3, 1–43.
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 15, 81–94.
Loomis, J. M., Blascovich, J. J., & Beall, A. C. (1999). Immersive virtual environment
Ressler, K. J., Rothbaum, B. O., Tannenbaum, L., Anderson, P., Graap, K., Zimand, E., et al.
technology as a basic research tool in psychology. Behavior Research Methods
(2004). Cognitive enhancers as adjuncts to psychotherapy. Use of D-cycloserine in
Instruments & Computers, 31(4), 557–564.
phobic individuals to facilitate extinction of fear. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61,
Lubow, R. E. (1973). Latent inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 79(6), 398–407.
Marks, I. M. (1969). Fears and phobias. New York: Academic Press.
Ritter, B. (1969a). Treatment of acrophobia with contact desensitization. Behaviour
Marks, I. M. (1987). The development of normal fear: a review. Journal of Child
Research and Therapy, 7(1), 41–45.
Psychology, 28, 667–697.
Ritter, B. (1969b). Use of contact desensitization, demonstration-plus-particitation and
Marshall, W. L. (1985). The effects of variable exposure to flooding therapy. Behavior
demonstration-alone in treatment of acrophobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
Therapy, 16, 117–135.
7(2), 157–164.
Marshall, W. L., Bristol, D., & Barbaree, H. E. (1992). Cognitions and courage in the
Rogers, B., & Graham, M. (1979). Motion parallax as an independent cue for depht-
avoidance behavior of acrophobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30(5), 463–
perception. Perception, 8(2), 125–134.
Rothbaum, B. O., Hodges, L., Alarcon, R., Ready, D., Shahar, F., Graap, K., et al. (1999).
Menzies, R. G. (1997). Height Phobia. In: G. L. C. Davey (Ed.), Phobias. A handbook of
Virtual reality exposure therapy for PTSD Vietnam veterans: a case study. Journal of
theory, research and treatmen (pp. 129–138). Chichester: Wiley.
Traumatic Stress, 12(2), 263–271.
Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1993). The etiology of fear of heights and its relationship
Rothbaum, B. O., Hodges, L., Watson, B., Kessler, G., & Opdyke, D. (1996). Virtual reality
to severity and individual response patterns. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31(4),
exposure therapy in the treatment of fear of flying: a case report. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 34, 447–481.
Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995a). Danger expectancies and insight in acrophobia.
Rothbaum, B. O., Hodges, L. F., Kooper, R., Opdyke, D., Williford, J. S., & North, M.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 2, 215–221.
(1995a). Effectiveness of computer-generated (virtual reality) graded exposure
Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995b). The etiology of acrophobia and its relationship to
in the treatment of acrophobia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(4), 626–
severity and individual-response patterns. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(7),
Rothbaum, B. O., Hodges, L. F., Kooper, R., Opdyke, D., Williford, J. S., & North, M.
Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1995c). The etiology of fear acrophobia and its relation-
(1995b). Virtual reality graded exposure in the treatment of acrophobia: a case
ship to severity and individual response patterns. Behaviour research and therapy,
report. Behaviour Therapy, 152(4), 626–628.
33(7), 795–803.
Roy, S. (2003). State of the art of virtual reality therapy (VRT) in phobic disorders.
Menzies, R. G., Harris, L. M., & Jones, M. K. (1998). Evidence from tree fearful samples for
PsychNology Journal, 1, 176–183.
a poor insight type in specific phobia. Depression and Anxiety, 8, 29–32.
Scarr, S., & Salapatek, P. (1970). Patterns of fear development during infancy. Merrill-
Menzies, R. G., & Parker, L. (2001). The origins of height fear: an evaluation of
Palmer Quarterly, 16, 53–90.
neoconditioning explanations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 185–199.
Schneider, J. W. (1982). Lens-assisted in vivo desensitization to heights. Journal of
Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1986). Immunization against observational conditioning of
Behaviour & Experimental Psychiatry, 13(4), 333–336.
snake fear in rhesus-monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(4), 307–318.
Schwartz, A., Campos, J., & Baisel, E. (1973). The visual cliff: cardiac and behavioral
Mineka, S., & Oehlberg, K. (2008). The relevance of recent developments in classical
correlates on the deep and shallow sides at five and nine months of age. Journal of
conditioning to understanding the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders.
Experimental Child Psychology, 15, 86–99.
Acta Psychologica, 127(3), 567–580.
Seligman, M. E. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behavior Therapy, 2(3), 307–320.
¨ hman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: the selective, automatic,
Simeonov, P. I., Hsiao, H. W., Dotson, B. W., & Ammons, D. E. (2005). Height effects in
and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 927–937.
real and virtual environments. Human Factors, 47(2), 430–438.
Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on the
Sutton-Simon, K., & Goldfried, M. R. (1979). Faulty thinking patterns in two types of
etiology of anxiety disorders—it's not what you thought it was. American Psychol-
anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 3(2), 193–203.
ogist, 61(1), 10–26.
Taylor, J. E., Deane, F. P., & Podd, J. V. (1999). Stability of driving fear acquisition
Morris, R. J., & Magrath, K. H. (1979). Contribution of therapist warmth to the contact
pathways over one year. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 927–939.
desessitization-treatment of acrophobia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
Teachman, B. A., Stefanucci, J. K., Clerkin, E. M., Cody, M. W., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008).
ogy, 47(4), 786–788.
A new mode of fear expression: perceptual bias in height fear. Emotion, 8(2),
Mowrer, O. H. (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a
reinforcing agent. Psychological Review, 46, 553–565.
Tolin, D. F., Lohr, J. M., Lee, T. C., & Sawchuk, C. N. (1999). Visual avoidance in specific
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., de Jong, P. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2002). The etiology of specific
phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 63–70.
fears and phobias in children: a critique of the non-associative account. Behaviour
Usher, J. A., & Neisser, U. (1993). Childhood amnesia and the beginnings of memory
Research and Therapy, 40, 185–195.
for our real life events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 155–
Muris, P., Schmidt, H., & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The structure of specific phobia
symptoms among children and adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37,
VanGerwen, L. J., Spinhoven, P., Diekstra, R. F. W., & VanDyck, R. (1997). People who
seek help for fear of flying: typology of flying phobics. Behavior Therapy, 28(2), 237–
Nakahara, H., Takemory, S., & Tsuruoka, H. (2000). Influence of height on the spatioal
orientation and equilibrium of the body. Otolaryngology-Head Neck Surgery, 123,
Wexler, M., & van Boxtel, J. J. A. (2005). Depth perception by the active observer. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 431–438.
C.M. Coelho et al. / Journal of Anxiety Disorders 23 (2009) 563–574
Whitney, S. L., Jacob, R. G., Sparto, P. J., Olshansky, E. F., Detweiler-Shostak, G., Brown, E.
Williams, S. W., & Watson, N. (1985). Perceived danger and perceived self-efficacy
L., et al. (2005). Acrophobia and pathological height vertigo: indications for
as cognitive determinants of acrophobic behavior. Behavior Therapy, 16,
vestibular physical therapy? Physical Therapy, 85, 443–458.
Wiederhold, B. K., & Wiederhold, M. D. (2005). Virtual reality therapy for anxiety
Witherington, D. C., Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Lejeune, L., & Seah, E. (2005).
disorders. Advances in evaluation and treatment. Washington: American Psycholo-
Avoidance of heights on the visual cliff in newly walking infants. Infancy, 7(3),
gical Association.
Williams, S. L., Dooseman, G., & Kleinfield, E. (1984). Comparative effectiveness of
Withers, R. D., & Deane, F. P. (1995). Origins of common fears: effects on severity,
guided mastery and exposure treatments for intractable phobias. Journal of Con-
anxiety responses and memories of onset. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(8),
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 505–518.
Williams, S. L., Turner, S. M., & Peer, D. F. (1985). Guided mastery and performance
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. California: Stanford University
desensitization treatments for severe acrophobia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Psychology, 53(2), 237–247.
Wolpe, J. (1969). The practice of behavior therapy. New York: Pergamon Press.
Source: http://www.virtualway.de/metaRubrik/The%20use%20of%20virtual%20reality%20in%20acrophobia%20research%20and%20treatment(1).pdf
ARTICLE IN PRESS Basic Research—Technology Comparative Evaluation of Antimicrobial Efficacy ofSodium Hypochlorite, MTAD, and Tetraclean AgainstEnterococcus faecalis Biofilm Luciano Giardino, MD, DDS,* Emanule Ambu, MD, DDS,† Enrico Savoldi, MD, DDS,*Roberto Rimondini, PhD, MS,‡ Clara Cassanelli, MD,§ and Eugenio A. Debbia, MD§ AbstractThe aim of this study was to compare the antimicrobialefficacy of 5.25% NaOCl, BioPure MTAD (Dentsply
An Alternative and soft tissue infections presenting at emergency rooms in the USA. During the period Approach in The from 1999 to 2005 the estimated number of hospitalizations involving S.aureus-related Treatment of Methicillin- infections increased 62% from 294,570 to Resistant Staphylococcus 477,927. In 2005, there were 11,406 S. aureus–